Play all audios:
ABSTRACT The symptom of “diminished interest or pleasure” in rewarding stimuli is an affective symptom of nicotine and amphetamine withdrawal, and a core symptom of depression. An
operational measure of this symptom is elevation of brain reward thresholds during drug withdrawal. We report here that acute co-administration of fluoxetine, a selective serotonin reuptake
inhibitor, and p-MPPI, a serotonin-1A receptor antagonist, alleviated the diminished interest in brain stimulation reward observed during withdrawal from nicotine or amphetamine in rats
(i.e., increased reward). By contrast, the same drug combination treatment did not reduce the somatic signs of nicotine withdrawal indicating symptom-specific neurobiological abnormalities.
Surprisingly, the same treatment had opposite effects in control rats where reductions in reward were produced, suggesting that animal models should be based primarily on studying specific
deficits that are pathognomic of a psychiatric disorder. The reversal of the affective aspects of drug withdrawal by a treatment that enhances serotonin neurotransmission indicates that
decreased serotonergic function may mediate the reward decrements characterizing nicotine and amphetamine withdrawal, and that these symptoms may be homologous to a core symptom of
non-drug–induced depressions. SIMILAR CONTENT BEING VIEWED BY OTHERS DOPAMINE D1-LIKE RECEPTOR BLOCKADE AND STIMULATION DECREASES OPERANT RESPONDING FOR NICOTINE AND FOOD IN MALE AND FEMALE
RATS Article Open access 19 August 2022 DIFFERENTIAL EFFECTS OF ACUTE AND CHRONIC ANTAGONIST AND AN IRREVERSIBLE ANTAGONIST TREATMENT ON COCAINE SELF-ADMINISTRATION BEHAVIOR IN RATS Article
Open access 24 May 2022 PRECLINICAL SUPPORT FOR THE THERAPEUTIC POTENTIAL OF ZOLMITRIPTAN AS A TREATMENT FOR COCAINE USE DISORDERS Article Open access 03 August 2020 MAIN Cessation of
chronic nicotine or amphetamine administration precipitates withdrawal syndromes characterized by affective symptoms, including “diminished interest or pleasure” in rewarding stimuli (i.e.,
anhedonia) (American Psychiatric Association 1994; Covey et al. 1998; Glassman 1993; Hughes 1992). Interestingly, the symptom of “diminished interest or pleasure” is not only a symptom of
drug withdrawal, but also a core symptom of depression and a negative symptom of schizophrenia (American Psychiatric Association 1994; Markou et al. 1998). Brain reward threshold elevation
is an operational measure of this symptom because it reflects diminished sensitivity to rewarding electrical stimuli. In rats, withdrawal from drugs of abuse belonging to diverse
pharmacological classes, such as nicotine (Epping-Jordan et al. 1998; Watkins et al. 2000b), amphetamine (Kokkinidis and Zacharko 1980a, 1980b; Kokkinidis et al. 1980, 1986; Leith and
Barrett 1976, 1980; Lin et al. 1999, 2000; Paterson et al. 2000; Wise and Munn 1995), cocaine (Baldo et al. 1999; Kokkinidis and McCarter 1990; Markou and Koob 1991, 1992a; Markou et al.
1992) morphine (Schulteis et al. 1994) and ethanol (Schulteis et al. 1995) elevated brain stimulation reward thresholds. In addition to the affective aspects of drug withdrawal reflected in
threshold elevations, nicotine, opiate, or ethanol withdrawal also lead to alterations in a set of behaviors termed somatic signs. In the case of nicotine, these somatic signs are primarily
gasps, writhes, eye blinks, and ptosis (Epping-Jordan et al. 1998; Hildebrand et al. 1997, 1999; Malin et al. 1992). It is unlikely that these somatic signs in the rat reflect the affective
component of drug withdrawal. Nevertheless, the study of both threshold elevations and somatic signs permits the investigation of the effects of manipulations on the various aspects of
withdrawal. Based on evidence demonstrating the efficacy of serotonergic antidepressant treatments, reduced cerebrospinal fluid levels of serotonin metabolites, endocrine measures reflecting
reduced serotonergic neurotransmission and the exacerbation of depressive symptomatology seen after serotonin (5-HT) depletion in depressed individuals, it is hypothesized that reduced
serotonergic neurotransmission underlies at least some aspects or some subtypes of non–drug-induced depressions (for reviews, see Caldecott-Hazard et al. 1991; Caldecott-Hazard and Schneider
1992; Heninger et al. 1996; Markou et al. 1998; Meltzer and Lowy 1988; Willner 1985). The purpose of the present study was to test the hypothesis that reduced serotonergic neurotransmission
mediates some of the affective aspects, not only of non-drug-induced depressions, but also of drug-induced depressions. Thus, the present study tested the hypothesis that enhancement of
serotonergic neurotransmission through acute administration of the selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SSRI) fluoxetine (Wong et al. 1995) in combination with a relatively selective
5-HT1A receptor antagonist would alleviate the symptom of “diminished interest or pleasure” observed in rats during nicotine or amphetamine withdrawal. Such an experimental outcome would
suggest that the affective aspects of drug withdrawal may be homologous to the core symptom of depression of “diminished interest or pleasure” in rewarding stimuli. It was also hypothesized
that this serotonergic drug treatment would not reverse the somatic signs of nicotine withdrawal that most likely do not reflect affective aspects of withdrawal. The combination treatment of
a SSRI together with the relatively selective 5-HT1A receptor antagonist p-MPPI [4-(2′-methoxy-phenyl)-1-[2′-(n-(2″-pyridinyl)-p-iodobenzamido]-ethyl-piperazine] (Allen et al. 1997; Kung et
al. 1994a, 1994b, 1995) was selected based on the following considerations. First, the threshold elevations associated with nicotine (Epping-Jordan et al. 1998) and amphetamine (Lin et al.
1999, 2000; Paterson et al. 2000) withdrawal, and the increases in somatic signs seen during nicotine withdrawal (Epping-Jordan et al. 1998) are relatively short lasting phenomena (3–5 days)
that do not allow the detection of the effects of pharmacological treatments that may require chronic administration for producing their effect. _In vivo_ microdialysis work demonstrated
that the administration of a SSRI together with a 5-HT1A receptor antagonist acutely and rapidly elevates forebrain serotonin dialysate levels beyond levels seen after acute treatment with
the SSRI alone (Auerbach and Hjorth 1995; Bel and Artigas 1993; Bengtsson and Milano 1996; Hjorth 1993, 1996; Invernizzi et al. 1994; Knobelman et al. 2000; Kreiss and Lucki 1994, 1995; for
reviews, see Artigas et al. 1996; Blier and de Montigny 1994). Thus, such a drug combination may have rapid therapeutic effects that may be detectable during the short-lasting drug
withdrawal. Second, the combination of a SSRI with pindolol [antagonist at 5-HT1A, 5-HT1B, and β-adrenergic receptors (Assie and Koek 1996; Bourin et al. 1998; Gobert and Millan 1999; Hoyer
and Schoeffter 1991; Newman-Tancredi et al. 1998); also reported to act as a partial agonist at 5-HT1A and α-adrenergic receptors (Clifford et al. 1998; Fornal et al. 1999a, 1999b, 1999c;
Gobert and Millan 1999; Pauwels and Palmier 1994a, 1994b)] accelerates the onset of antidepressant action of SSRIs in humans (Bordet et al. 1998; Zanardi et al. 1997, 1998), or augments the
antidepressant response to SSRIs in terms of both magnitude and duration of the response (Maes et al. 1999; Perez et al. 1997; Tome and Isaac 1998; for review, see McAskill et al. 1998;
however, see Berman et al. 1997, 1999; Tome et al. 1997a, 1997b). In the present study, instead of pindolol, which has multiple receptor actions, the relatively selective 5-HT1A receptor
antagonist p-MPPI was used. The use of p-MPPI allowed the investigation of the role of serotonergic neurotransmission and antagonism at 5-HT1A receptors in drug withdrawal, without
complicating data interpretation with neuroadaptive effects that may occur with chronic SSRI treatment. In summary, acute treatment with a SSRI together with a 5-HT1A receptor antagonist
allowed us to rapidly elevate extracellular serotonin levels and delineate the potential role of serotonin neurotransmission in the affective and somatic aspects of drug withdrawal. MATERIAL
AND METHODS SUBJECTS Male Wistar rats (Charles River, Hollister, CA) (300–320 g at the start of the experiment) were housed in pairs in a temperature and humidity controlled environment
with a 12 hr light/dark cycle (lights on at 6:00 A.M.). Food and water were available ad libitum in the home cages. All subjects were treated in accordance with the National Institutes of
Health “Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals,” and the animal facilities and experimental protocols were in accordance with the Association for the Assessment and Accreditation
of Laboratory Animal Care. All testing was conducted during the light phase of the light/dark cycle. INTRACRANIAL SELF-STIMULATION The intracranial self-stimulation task used is a modified
version (Markou and Koob 1992b, 1993) of a discrete-trial current-threshold procedure originally developed by Kornetsky and co-workers (Kornetsky and Esposito 1979). The apparatus, surgery,
procedure, and parameters for the lateral hypothalamic intracranial self-stimulation task have been described in detail previously (Harrison et al. 1999; Kornetsky and Esposito 1979; Markou
and Koob 1992b, 1993). _APPARATUS_ The experimental apparatus consisted of 16 Plexiglas chambers (30.5 × 30 × 17 cm) (Med Associates Inc.) encased in sound-attenuating boxes (San Diego
Instruments, San Diego, CA). Each operant chamber consisted of a stainless steel grid floor and a metal wheel manipulandum located on one wall, which required a 0.2 N force to rotate it a
quarter turn. Gold-contact swivel commutators and bipolar leads connected the animals to the stimulation circuit (Plastics One, Roanoke, VA). Brain stimulation was administered by constant
current stimulators (Stimtek 1200, San Diego Instruments, San Diego, CA). The stimulation parameters, data collection and all programming functions were controlled by a microcomputer.
_SURGERY_ The rats were prepared with 11 mm stainless steel bipolar electrodes (Plastics One; diameter = 0.25 mm) in the posterior lateral hypothalamus (AP −0.5 mm from bregma; L ± 1.7 mm;
DV −8.3 mm from dura, with the incisor bar set at 5 mm above the inter-aural line; Pellegrino et al. 1979) under halothane anesthesia (1–1.5% halothane/oxygen mixture) and allowed to recover
for at least seven days. INTRACRANIAL SELF-STIMULATION PROCEDURE The subjects were initially trained to turn the wheel manipulandum on a fixed ratio 1 schedule of reinforcement during which
each quarter turn of a wheel manipulandum resulted in the delivery of a contingent electrical reinforcer. The electrical reinforcer had a train duration of 500 msec and consisted of 0.1
msec rectangular cathodal pulses that were delivered with 100 Hz frequency. The current intensity was adjusted for each animal and typically ranged from 100-250 μA. After successful
familiarization with this procedure (two sessions of 100 reinforcers in less than 20 min), the rats were gradually trained on the discrete-trial current threshold procedure. In this
procedure, at the start of each trial, rats received a non-contingent electrical stimulus. During the following 7.5 sec, the limited hold, if the subjects responded by turning the wheel
manipulandum a quarter turn (positive response), they received a second contingent stimulus identical to the previous non-contingent stimulus. During a 2 sec period immediately after a
positive response, further responses had no reinforcement or task consequences. If no response occurred during the 7.5 sec limited hold period, a negative response was recorded. The
intertrial interval (ITI), which followed the limited hold period, had an average duration of 10 sec (ranging from 7.5–12.5 sec). Responses that occurred during the ITI resulted in a further
12.5 sec delay of the onset of the next trial. During training, the duration of the ITI and delay periods imposed by inappropriate ITI responding were gradually increased until the standard
task parameters were reached. Stimulation intensities were varied according to the classical psychophysical method of limits. Thus, the subjects received four alternating series of
ascending and descending current intensities starting with a descending series. Within each series the stimulus intensity was altered by 5 μA steps between each set of trials (three trials
per set). The initial stimulus intensity was set at 40 μA above the baseline current threshold for each animal. A series was terminated after either 15 stimulus increments (or decrements)
had occurred, or after the determination of the threshold for the series (see below). Each test session typically lasted 30 min and provided two dependent variables. After training in the
above brain stimulation procedure, rats were tested until stable baseline thresholds and latencies were achieved (±10% over a 5-day period), which typically occurred after two to three weeks
of daily baseline testing. _REWARD THRESHOLDS_ The current threshold for each descending series was defined as the stimulus intensity between the successful completion of a set of trials
(positive responses during two or more of the three trials) and the stimulus intensity for the first set of trials, of two consecutive sets, during which the animal failed to respond
positively on two or more of the three trials. During the ascending series, the threshold was defined as the stimulus intensity between the unsuccessful completion of a set of trials
(negative responses during two or more of the three trials) and the stimulus intensity for the first set of trials, of two consecutive sets, during which the animal responded positively on
two or more of the trials. Thus, during each test session, four thresholds were determined and the mean of these values was taken as the threshold for each subject. _RESPONSE LATENCY_ The
latency between the onset of the non-contingent stimulus at the start of each trial and a positive response was recorded as the response latency. The response latency for each test session
was defined as the mean response latency of all trials during which a positive response occurred. DRUGS d-Amphetamine sulphate (obtained from the National Institute on Drug Abuse, Bethesda,
MD) and fluoxetine hydrochloride (Research Biochemicals Inc., Natick, MA) were dissolved in saline and administered intraperitoneally in a volume of 1 ml/kg.
4-(2′-Methoxy-phenyl)-1-[2′-(n-(2″-pyridinyl)-p-iodobenzamido]-ethyl-piperazine hydrochloride (p-MPPI) (Research Biochemicals Inc.) was dissolved in sterile water and sonicated for 10–20
min, and then brought to a pH of approximately 5.2 with 0.1 M NaOH. p-MPPI hydrochloride was administered subcutaneously in a volume of 4 ml/kg. Nicotine hydrogen tartrate (Sigma, St. Louis,
MO) was dissolved in saline and administered through subcutaneous osmotic minipumps. Mecamylamine HCl (Sigma) was dissolved in saline and administered subcutaneously in a volume of 1 ml/kg.
NICOTINE ADMINISTRATION AND WITHDRAWAL When stable self-stimulation performance had been achieved, osmotic minipumps [model 2ML1 (7 day) Alza Corporation, Palo Alto, CA] filled with either
saline (4 groups of _n_ = 8) or nicotine hydrogen tartrate dissolved in saline (4 groups of _n_ = 8), were implanted subcutaneously under halothane/oxygen anesthesia. The surgical wounds
were closed with 9 mm stainless steel autoclips (Becton Dickinson Primary Care Diagnostics, Sparks, MD). The concentration of nicotine was adjusted to compensate for differences in body
weight at the time of implantation, resulting in a dose of 9 mg/kg/day nicotine tartrate (or 3.16 mg/kg/day base) for 7 days. During these 7 days, intracranial self-stimulation reward
thresholds, response latencies and the body weight of the subjects were recorded daily. The minipumps were removed on the seventh day after pump implantation. Intracranial self-stimulation
reward thresholds, response latencies, the number of somatic signs of withdrawal and body weight were measured at regular intervals thereafter. Subjects were tested in the intracranial
self-stimulation (ICSS) procedure at 6, 12, 18, 24, 36, 48, 72, 96, 120, and 144 hr after pump removal. Body weight was measured immediately before each ICSS session. The somatic signs of
nicotine withdrawal have been described in detail previously (Epping-Jordan et al. 1998; Hildebrand et al. 1997, 1999; Malin et al. 1992; Watkins et al. 2000b). Briefly, the number of
abstinence signs, including gasps, writhes, eye blinks, chewing, teeth chattering, cheek tremor, ptosis, “wet dog” shakes, piloerection, genital grooming, and escapes were recorded during a
10 min period. Somatic signs were assessed immediately after the self-stimulation sessions at 6.5, 12.5, 18.5, 24.5, 36.5, 48.5, 72.5, 96.5, 120.5, and 144.5 hr after pump removal. Acute
administration of vehicle, p-MPPI hydrochloride (3 mg/kg), fluoxetine hydrochloride (5 mg/kg), or p-MPPI hydrochloride (3 mg/kg) + fluoxetine hydrochloride (5 mg/kg), occurred prior to the
18-hr test session. This testing time point was selected based on the time course of threshold elevations and somatic signs observed during nicotine withdrawal previously (Epping-Jordan et
al. 1998). p-MPPI hydrochloride was administered 135 min prior to test, and fluoxetine hydrochloride was administered 120 min prior to test. _MECAMYLAMINE CHALLENGE_ On the seventh day
post-pump removal, all subjects were subcutaneously injected with mecamylamine (3.0 mg/kg) 15 min before intracranial self-stimulation testing, which was immediately followed by assessment
of somatic signs of withdrawal. The purpose of this mecamylamine challenge was to test whether prior nicotine exposure and withdrawal would lead to potential tolerance or augmentation to the
withdrawal effects. The dose of mecamylamine used was selected based on data showing that this dose reliably precipitated nicotine withdrawal symptomatology in rats that were chronically
treated with nicotine (Watkins et al. 2000b). AMPHETAMINE ADMINISTRATION AND WITHDRAWAL The amphetamine administration regimen was a modification of that used originally by Leith and Barrett
(1976), and identical to that used by Lin et al. (1999). Briefly, d-amphetamine sulphate (6 groups of _n_ = 8) was administered intraperitoneally three times a day (6:00 A.M., 12:00 A.M.,
6:00 P.M.) for four days in a rising dose regime starting at 1 mg/kg and stabilizing at 5 mg/kg (i.e., 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 5, 5, 5, 5, 5, 5, 5 mg/kg; total dose = 50 mg/kg). Another set of rats
(6 groups of _n_ = 8) was injected at the same time-points with saline. Intracranial self-stimulation reward thresholds and response latencies were determined at 12, 36, 42, 60, 84, 108,
132, and 156 hr after the last amphetamine or saline injection. Acute administration of vehicle, p-MPPI hydrochloride (3 mg/kg), fluoxetine hydrochloride (2.5 or 5 mg/kg), or p-MPPI
hydrochloride (3 mg/kg) + fluoxetine hydrochloride (2.5 or 5 mg/kg) was administered prior to the 36-hr test session. This testing time point was selected based on the time course of
threshold elevations observed during amphetamine withdrawal previously (Lin et al. 1999). p-MPPI hydrochloride was administered 135 minutes prior to test, and fluoxetine hydrochloride was
administered 120 minutes prior to test. DATA ANALYSES _CHRONIC NICOTINE_ Body weight data recorded during the nicotine phase were expressed as a percentage of the weight immediately after
minipump implantation, to take into account the weight of the mini-pump. Body weight, intracranial self-stimulation reward threshold and response latency data were analyzed using two-way
analyses of variance (ANOVAs) with time after minipump implantation as the within-subject factor and chronic drug treatment (nicotine or saline) as the between-subject factor. _NICOTINE AND
AMPHETAMINE WITHDRAWAL_ All reward threshold and response latency data were expressed as a percentage of the mean baseline value assessed during the five days immediately prior to the
implantation of the minipumps or the first amphetamine injection. Body weight data recorded during the nicotine withdrawal phase were expressed as a percentage of the body weight immediately
after minipump removal. Data were analyzed using three-way mixed factors analyses of variance. The within-subjects factor was time after nicotine, amphetamine or saline treatment, and the
two between-subjects factors were chronic drug treatment [drug (nicotine or amphetamine) versus saline] and acute drug treatment administered during withdrawal. Statistically significant
interactions were followed by post-hoc Newman-Keuls tests. The level of significance was set at _p_ < .05. _MECAMYLAMINE CHALLENGE_ Reward threshold, response latency and somatic signs
data were compared to the data collected the previous day (time-point 144 hr in withdrawal, when performance had returned to baseline levels for all subjects). The data were analyzed using
three-way mixed factors analyses of variance. The within-subjects factor was dose of mecamylamine (0 or 3.0 mg/kg), and the two between-subjects factors were chronic drug treatment (nicotine
or saline) and acute drug treatment administered during withdrawal. Statistically significant interactions were followed by post-hoc Newman-Keuls tests. The level of significance was set at
_p_ < .05. RESULTS CHRONIC NICOTINE There were no statistically significant differences between the mean baseline thresholds or response latencies of subjects assigned to the saline
“withdrawal” group (_n_ = 32) [mean thresholds ± SEM: 155.55 ± 10.06 μA; mean latencies ± SEM: 3.34 ± 0.06 sec], and subjects assigned to the nicotine group (_n_ = 32) [mean thresholds ±
SEM: 160.36 ± 8.04 μA; mean latencies ± SEM: 3.36 ± 0.04 sec] (_p_ > .1). Based on the animals’ performance during the 6- and 12-hr tests (time-points before the acute drug administration
during withdrawal) subjects were assigned to treatment groups so that original withdrawal effects on threshold elevations were equal across groups. _REWARD THRESHOLDS_ Reward thresholds of
nicotine-exposed animals were significantly lower than those of saline-exposed animals [F(1,62) = 5.983, _p_ < .05] during pump-exposure. Post-hoc analysis of a significant days X drug
interaction revealed that the thresholds of nicotine-exposed animals were lower than those of saline-exposed control animals for the first two days after minipump implantation [F(5,310) =
6.166, _p_ < .01] (see Figure 1a ). _RESPONSE LATENCIES_ Response latencies of nicotine-exposed rats also were shorter than those of saline-exposed control rats during pump-exposure
[F(1,62) = 7.917, _p_ < .05]. A trend for a statistically significant days × drug interaction [F(5,310) = 2.134, _p_ = .0612] indicated that the difference in response latencies between
nicotine and saline controls was greatest during the first two days of treatment (see Figure 1b). _BODY WEIGHT_ The percent body weights of the nicotine-exposed rats were lower than those of
the saline control rats during the nicotine phase [F(1,62) = 21.551, _p_ < .01]. The percent body weight of both nicotine-exposed and control rats increased during the seven days of pump
pressure [F(6,372) = 77.247, _p_ < .01]. However, a significant days × drug interaction [F(6,372) = 5.36, _p_ < .01] revealed that the body weight of the saline control rats increased
steadily over time, whereas the nicotine-exposed rats did not gain weight during the first five days of nicotine-exposure. Consequently, the percent body weights of the saline-exposed rats
were consistently greater than those of the nicotine-exposed rats during the seven day pump exposure (_p_ < .05) (see Table 1). NICOTINE WITHDRAWAL _REWARD THRESHOLDS_ Withdrawal from
chronic nicotine administration reliably increased reward thresholds compared to saline-treated rats [F(1,56) = 57.87, _p_ < .0001] (see Figure 2). Analysis of the significant time ×
chronic treatment × acute treatment interaction [F(27,504) = 2.55, _p_ < .0001] revealed the following. Nicotine-exposed rats treated with vehicle during withdrawal had elevated
thresholds at withdrawal hr 6, 12, 18, 24 and 36, and returned to baseline levels at 48 hr post-pump removal compared to saline-exposed rats treated with vehicle. Administration of p-MPPI (3
mg/kg) or fluoxetine (5 mg/kg) administered alone had no significant effect on thresholds of either saline- or nicotine-exposed subjects (Figure 2a, b, d, e). By contrast, nicotine-exposed
rats treated with p-MPPI + fluoxetine had significantly lower thresholds than the nicotine-exposed vehicle-treated subjects at 24, 36 and 48 hr post-nicotine (Figure 2f); that is, thresholds
returned to baseline levels 8 hours after the acute combination drug treatment, and 24 hr before nicotine-exposed vehicle-treated subjects’ thresholds returned to baseline levels.
Saline-exposed subjects treated with vehicle during withdrawal exhibited stable thresholds over the duration of the experiment (Figure 2a). Administration of p-MPPI or fluoxetine alone had
no significant effect on reward thresholds of saline-exposed rats (Figure 2a, b). By contrast, in saline-exposed rats, the co-administration of p-MPPI + fluoxetine elevated thresholds
compared to the previously stable performance of this group at the time-point immediately after this acute drug administration (Figure 2c), an effect that was seen repeatedly (see below).
Thresholds returned to baseline levels by the following test session that was 8 hours after the acute drug treatment. _RESPONSE LATENCIES_ Although there were no statistically significant
interactions [F = 0.94–1.48, _p_ > .1], there was a main effect of chronic drug treatment [F(1,56) = 7.64, _p_ < .01], and a main effect of time [F(9,504) = 7.05, _p_ <.0001] on
response latencies. These main effects indicate longer response latencies in nicotine-exposed rats, and in both groups at 24 hr post-pump removal (see Figure 3). _SOMATIC SIGNS_
Nicotine-exposed animals exhibited an increased number of somatic signs of withdrawal for 72 hr after pump removal compared to saline-exposed rats, reflected in a significant chronic drug
treatment × time interaction [F(9,504) = 14.78, _p_ < .0001] (Figure 4). None of the acute drug treatments administered during withdrawal had a significant effect on the number of somatic
signs in either saline- or nicotine-exposed subjects (_p_ > .1). _BODY WEIGHT_ During nicotine withdrawal, animals that received the p-MPPI + fluoxetine combination treatment had
significantly lower percent body weights than the animals in other treatment groups [F(3,56) = 2.951, _p_ < .05]. A significant hours × pump interaction [F(9,504) = 6.63, _p_ < .01]
revealed that only the nicotine withdrawing animals significantly increased body weight only between 96–120 hr and 120–144 hr post-pump removal. The percent body weight of the
nicotine-exposed rats was therefore greater than that of saline-exposed animals at time points 120 and 144 hr post-pump removal (see Table 2). MECAMYLAMINE CHALLENGE Mecamylamine had no
significant effect on reward thresholds [F(1,56) = 2.974, n.s.]; (0 mg/kg mean ± SEM = 102.287 ± 9.132; 3.0 mg/kg mean ± SEM = 104.288 ± 9.352). Response latencies of all subjects were
increased by the administration of mecamylamine [F(1,56) = 5.598, _p_ < .05], (0 mg/kg mean ± SEM = 100.109 ± 0.954; 3.0 mg/kg mean ± SEM = 102.973 ± 1.229). Mecamylamine also increased
the number of somatic signs displayed by all subjects [F(1,56) = 308.443, _p_ < .01], (0 mg/kg mean ± SEM = 2.453 ± 1.967; 3.0 mg/kg mean ± SEM = 13.172 ± 4.914), thus not showing
differential sensitivity of nicotine- and saline-exposed rats to this dose of mecamylamine. AMPHETAMINE WITHDRAWAL There were no statistically significant differences between the mean
baseline thresholds or response latencies of subjects assigned to the saline “withdrawal” group (_n_ = 48) [mean thresholds ± SEM: 158.86 ± 8.67 μA; mean latencies ± SEM: 3.36 ± 0.044 sec],
and subjects assigned to the amphetamine withdrawal group (_n_ = 48) [mean thresholds ± SEM: 146.03 ± 4.26 μA; mean latencies ± SEM: 3.30 ± 0.04 sec] (_p_ > .1). Based on the animals’
performance during the 12-hr test (time-point before the acute drug administration during withdrawal), subjects were assigned to treatment groups so that original withdrawal effects on
threshold elevations were equal across groups. _THRESHOLDS_ Amphetamine-exposed subjects exhibited elevated brain reward thresholds relative to saline-treated rats after the final
amphetamine injection [F(1,84) = 106.76, _p_ < .0001) (Figure 5). Analysis of the significant time × chronic treatment × acute treatment interaction [F(35,588) = 2.56, _p_ < .0001]
revealed the following. Amphetamine-exposed animals treated with vehicle prior to the 36-hr time-point had elevated thresholds at withdrawal hours 12, 36, 44, 60, and 84, and returned to
baseline levels at 108 hr after the last amphetamine injection compared to saline-exposed rats treated with vehicle. p-MPPI (3 mg/kg) or fluoxetine (2.5 mg/kg) administered alone had no
significant effect on the thresholds of either saline- or amphetamine-exposed subjects (Figure 5a, b, d, e). Nevertheless, the higher fluoxetine dose (5 mg/kg) reduced the duration of the
amphetamine withdrawal-induced elevations of thresholds by 24 hr (Figure 5e). Most importantly, administration of p-MPPI and either of the two doses of fluoxetine prior to the 36-hr time
point reduced the duration of the threshold elevations in amphetamine-exposed rats (Figure 5f). More specifically, amphetamine-exposed rats treated with p-MPPI + 2.5 mg/kg fluoxetine had
significantly lower thresholds than amphetamine-exposed vehicle-treated subjects at 60 and 84 hours post-amphetamine (24 hr after the acute treatment), while p-MPPI + 5 mg/kg fluoxetine
returned thresholds to baseline levels at the 42-hr time-point (8 hr after the acute treatment) (Figure 5f). That is, 2.5 mg/kg fluoxetine in combination with p-MPPI returned thresholds to
baseline levels 48 hours earlier than vehicle treatment, while 5 mg/kg fluoxetine in combination with p-MPPI returned thresholds to baseline levels 72 hours earlier than vehicle treatment
(Figure 5f). As expected, saline-exposed subjects treated with vehicle exhibited stable thresholds over the duration of the experiment (Figure 5a). Administration of p-MPPI or either of the
two doses of fluoxetine alone had no significant effect on reward thresholds of saline-exposed rats (Figure 5a, b). As seen in the nicotine withdrawal experiment, in saline-treated rats, the
co-administration of p-MPPI + the higher dose of fluoxetine (5 mg/kg) elevated thresholds compared to the previously stable thresholds of this group and the saline-exposed vehicle-treated
group's thresholds immediately after the acute drug administration (Figure 5c). Thresholds returned to baseline levels by the following test session that was 8 hours after the acute
drug treatment. Other experiments in unperturbed non-withdrawing control rats confirmed that 10 mg/kg p-MPPI combined with 5 mg/kg fluoxetine also significantly elevated thresholds (data not
shown), providing the third demonstration of decrease in reward after this drug combination when subjects are not in drug withdrawal. _RESPONSE LATENCIES_ There was a main effect of acute
drug treatment during withdrawal [F(5,84) = 2.88, _p_ < .05], a main effect of time [F(7,588) = 5.87, _p_ < .0001], and a significant time X acute drug treatment interaction [F(35,588)
= 1.96, _p_ < .001]. These effects reflect longer response latencies in both saline and amphetamine-exposed rats immediately after the co-administration of p-MPPI and either of the two
fluoxetine doses (36 hr time-point). Further, the effects of p-MPPI + the higher dose of fluoxetine (5 mg/kg) on response latencies were still observed at the 42 hour time-point of
withdrawal which was 8 hr after the administration of the combination drug treatment (see Figure 6). DISCUSSION During chronic nicotine or saline administration, animals treated with
nicotine had lower brain reward thresholds than saline controls. The small threshold elevation of saline-exposed rats on the first day of exposure to the minipumps may be due to residual
discomfort from the minipump surgery that occurred the previous day. Similar small and transient threshold elevations after minipump surgery have been observed previously (Paterson et al.
2000). This effect was not observed in nicotine-exposed rats, possibly reflecting a mild analgesic effect of nicotine. Analgesic effects of nicotine that show rapid tolerance have been shown
previously with bolus injections of nicotine (1–3 mg/kg, free base) or with continuous nicotine exposure through tobacco smoke or minipumps containing almost double the nicotine dose (6
mg/kg/day free base = approximately 17 mg/kg/day salt) used in the present study (Marubio et al. 1999; Mousa et al. 1988; Yang et al. 1992). On the second day of minipump exposure,
nicotine-exposed animals still had lower thresholds than saline controls, indicating a small reward-enhancing effect of continuous slow subcutaneous infusion of nicotine. This result is
consistent with increases in brain stimulation reward seen after acute bolus nicotine administration (Bauco and Wise 1994; Clarke and Kumar 1984; Huston-Lyons and Kornetsky 1992).
Nevertheless, in the present study, this small reward-enhancing effect of nicotine was no longer evident on the third day of minipump exposure possibly reflecting tolerance to the
reward-enhancing effects of slowly infused nicotine. This rapid tachyphylaxis is consistent with experimental and anecdotal reports in humans indicating that the first cigarette of the day
is the most pleasurable, in part because of acute tolerance that develops to the subjective effects of nicotine over the day as the smoker consumes additional cigarettes (for review, see
Russell 1989). The effects of continuous nicotine infusion on response latencies followed a similar pattern of results to that of thresholds. Nicotine-exposed rats were faster in responding
than saline-exposed rats, and there was a trend for this effect to be larger during the first two days of exposure to nicotine. Finally, consistent with previous reports, chronic nicotine
administration suppressed body weight gain compared to the weight gain of saline-exposed animals (Carroll et al. 1989; Grunberg et al. 1986; Levin et al. 1987). After removal of the
minipump, animals previously exposed to nicotine gained weight on the last two days of measurement (120 and 144 hr post-pump removal) in comparison to both their own baseline body weight and
that of saline-exposed animals. Previous studies indicated similar, but more dramatic, effects of nicotine withdrawal on body weight after longer nicotine exposure than that used in the
present study (Levin et al. 1987; Winders and Grunberg 1989). None of the acute serotonergic drug treatments affected this pattern of weight gain in either saline- or nicotine-exposed rats.
In replication of previous findings (Epping-Jordan et al. 1998; Kokkinidis and Zacharko 1980a, 1980b; Kokkinidis et al. 1986; Leith and Barrett 1976, 1980; Lin et al. 1999, 2000; Paterson et
al. 2000; Watkins et al. 2000b; Wise and Munn 1995), both nicotine and amphetamine withdrawal resulted in decreased reward reflected in elevated brain reward thresholds relative to
saline-treated rats’ and to their own pre-drug baseline thresholds. As anticipated (Epping-Jordan et al. 1998; Hildebrand et al. 1997, 1999; Malin et al. 1992; Watkins et al. 2000b),
nicotine withdrawal also led to significant increases in somatic signs. Most importantly, the results indicated that the co-administration of fluoxetine, a selective serotonin reuptake
inhibitor, and p-MPPI, a 5-HT1A receptor antagonist, alleviated the diminished interest in rewarding stimuli observed during withdrawal from nicotine or amphetamine in rats. Further, the
reversal of the threshold elevations associated with amphetamine withdrawal was dose-related because the co-administration of the 5-HT1A antagonist with the highest fluoxetine dose returned
thresholds to baseline levels 24 hr earlier than the co-administration of p-MPPI with the lowest fluoxetine dose. Neither p-MPPI nor the low dose of fluoxetine alone had an effect on
nicotine or amphetamine withdrawal-induced threshold elevations; while the highest dose of fluoxetine when administered alone reduced the duration of the amphetamine, but not nicotine,
withdrawal-induced elevations of thresholds by 24 hr. This rapid restoration of the sensitivity to electrical stimulation (i.e., relative increase in reward) may be attributable to increased
serotonin function in forebrain structures, such as the frontal cortex, the hippocampus and the striatum (e.g., Bel and Artigas 1993; Dreshfield et al. 1996, 1997; Invernizzi et al. 1994;
Gobert and Millan 1999; Rutter et al. 1994). The present data are consistent with the hypothesis that the rapid onset of clinical antidepressant action of SSRIs when combined with pindolol
(Bordet et al. 1998; Tome et al. 1997a, 1997b; Zanardi et al. 1997, 1998; however, see Berman et al. 1997, 1999) is partly attributable to pindolol's 5-HT1A antagonist properties.
Nevertheless, other receptors, such as the 5-HT1B and α-adrenergic receptors, for which pindolol has antagonist and/or partial agonist properties, and other neuromechanisms may also
contribute to pindolol's augmentation of SSRI antidepressant effects. By contrast, in saline-treated rats, this combination treatment resulted in a small, but statistically significant
elevation of reward thresholds (i.e., decrease in reward) compared to the previously stable performance of these groups, an effect that was seen repeatedly (see below). Considering the
relatively short-lasting elevation of reward thresholds seen in saline-treated subjects after the administration of the drug combination, which is typical of acute drug treatments, the
permanent reversal of the reward threshold elevations of nicotine- and amphetamine-withdrawing subjects after a single treatment is note-worthy and somewhat surprising. It is possible that
an acute drug treatment was sufficient to permanently reverse the threshold elevations for the following reasons. The combination of fluoxetine's long half-life (up to 7 hr with
detection of fluoxetine levels up to 30 hr after systemic administration of doses similar to the ones used here; Caccia et al. 1990) and the relatively short-lasting threshold elevation seen
especially during nicotine withdrawal may have allowed the gradually declining enhancement of serotonergic neurotransmission to coincide with the gradual return of reward thresholds to
baseline levels. Further, the doses administered in the present study are much higher than those that would be tolerated by humans on the first administration (although such comparisons are
hard to make) rendering it more likely that the drug treatment would have an acute effect. Further, it has been argued that antidepressants may produce immediate improvement of _some_
symptoms in humans, but this acute effect may be hard to detect statistically because the improvement may be initially small and seen only in some, but not all, symptoms (Detke et al. 1997;
Frazer 1994). Another interesting possibility is that once reversal of a deficit occurs in a relatively “healthy” animal in which the deficit was transiently induced by a manipulation, a
“healthy” system may not readily regress to the previous imbalance. Considering that chronic drug treatment is required in most cases before a therapeutic antidepressant effect is observed
in humans, the demonstration of the reversal of a reward deficit with a single acute drug treatment in the present study may be considered a limitation of the proposed animal model. It could
be argued that under such conditions the mechanisms leading to the reversal of the behavioral deficit are not the same as the ones leading to the clinical therapeutic effect (Detke et al.
1997). Even though such arguments have merit, there are counter-arguments and reasons why the study of the acute effects of drugs, when guided by good working hypotheses, still could promote
our understanding of drug withdrawal, depressive symptomatology, and mode of action of antidepressant compounds. The present study was designed to test the hypothesis that activation of a
specific neurobiological mechanism (i.e., enhanced serotonergic neurotransmission) would reverse a reward deficit. The findings offer one of the few experimental demonstrations that the
enhanced serotonin neurotransmission induced by such drug treatments leads to observable behavioral changes with relevance to depressive symptomatology. For example, the _acute_
administration of the 5-HT1A partial receptor agonist buspirone enhanced the antidepressant-like actions of a low _acute_ dose of a SSRI that had no effect on the forced swim stress when
administered on its own (Redrobe and Bourin 1998). By contrast, the addition of a 5-HT1A receptor antagonist to the chronic treatment with a SSRI did _not_ accelerate the onset of
antidepressant-like action seen after chronic treatment with the SSRI alone neither in the olfactory bulbectomized rat model of depression (Cryan et al. 1998, 1999) nor in the forced swim
test (Moser and Sanger 1999). Finally, the study of the _acute_ effects of drugs in the forced swim test, a postulated animal model of depression (e.g., for review, see Lucki 1997), and in
deficits of the prepulse inhibition of startle, a postulated animal model of gating deficits seen in several psychiatric disorders (e.g., for review, see Swerdlow and Geyer 1998), has led to
a plethora of neurobiological findings with relevance to both healthy and psychopathological states. The ability of the two above-mentioned behavioral paradigms to predict the therapeutic
efficacy of antidepressant and antipsychotic compounds, respectively, demonstrates the predictive validity of these models, which is a starting point in the development of any model (Geyer
and Markou 1995). Similarly, the reversal of the affective, but not the somatic, aspects of drug withdrawal with a drug treatment that enhances serotonergic neurotransmission indicates some
predictive and discriminant validity of the present paradigm as an animal model of the symptom of “diminished interest or pleasure”. Ample evidence indicates that brain stimulation reward
thresholds are a valid and reliable measure of reward that is minimally affected by performance manipulations (Markou and Koob 1992b, 1993). Further, extensive previous work with the
identical brain stimulation reward procedure used in the present study demonstrated that reward thresholds do _not_ co-vary with response latencies (e.g., Harrison et al. 1999; Lin et al.
1999, 2000; Macey et al. 2000; Markou and Koob 1991, 1992b; Paterson et al. 2000). Similarly, the present results also demonstrated a lack of co-variation of threshold elevations with
increased response latencies (see Results section and Figure 3 and 6). Thus, threshold elevations seen during drug withdrawal cannot be attributed to effects of the various manipulations on
performance. The absence of an effect of doses of fluoxetine up to 5 mg/kg on reward thresholds in control subjects in the present and previous studies in our laboratory (Lin et al. 1999)
appears to be in contrast to another report demonstrating elevations in thresholds after 2.5–20 mg/kg fluoxetine using the same intracranial self-stimulation procedure (Lee and Kornetsky
1998). The reasons for this discrepancy are not clear, although there are some methodological differences between the two studies. In the present study, electrodes were placed in the medial
forebrain bundle at the level of the posterior lateral hypothalamus, whereas the majority of placements in the Lee and Kornetsky (1998) study were in the ventral tegmental area. Moreover, in
the present study, fluoxetine was administered 2 hr, while in the Lee and Kornetsky study 1 hr prior to test. Finally, administration of 10 mg/kg fluoxetine 2 hr before the test did result
in threshold elevations in our laboratory (Harrison and Markou, unpublished observations). Thus, the differences in results appear to be quantitative (i.e., dose) rather than qualitative.
Taken together, these findings suggest that higher acute doses of fluoxetine than those used in the present study may counteract threshold elevations associated with drug withdrawal. The
present data suggest that reduced serotonergic neurotransmission may be one of the neurobiological abnormalities underlying the symptom of “diminished interest or pleasure” seen in both
amphetamine and nicotine withdrawal in humans (American Psychiatric Association 1994; Covey et al. 1998; Glassman 1993; Hughes 1992; Markou et al. 1998). This hypothesis is consistent with
the observation that withdrawal from other drugs of abuse, such as cocaine (Parsons et al. 1995) or ethanol (Weiss et al. 1996), also is characterized by decreased serotonergic
neurotransmission as reflected in decreased _in vivo_ dialysate serotonin levels. Further, considering the strong evidence implicating reduced serotonergic neurotransmission as one of the
neurochemical abnormalities associated with depression (for reviews, see Caldecott-Hazard et al. 1991; Caldecott-Hazard and Schneider 1992; Heninger et al. 1996; Markou et al. 1998; Meltzer
and Lowy 1988; Willner 1985), it can be hypothesized that there is homology between the symptom of “diminished interest or pleasure” seen in individuals withdrawing from drugs of abuse and
the same symptom seen in depressed patients. Decreased dopaminergic neurotransmission is another neurobiological abnormality common to withdrawal from a variety of drugs of abuse, including
nicotine (Hildebrand et al. 1999; Parsons et al. 1995; Weiss et al. 1996). Based on the above hypothesis of homology between symptoms seen across psychiatric diagnostic categories, it can be
postulated that reward deficits associated with withdrawal from drugs of abuse may be an animal model of the symptom of “diminished interest or pleasure” with construct, convergent, and
predictive validities (Geyer and Markou 1995). In addition to reward threshold elevations, amphetamine withdrawal also is characterized by decreased breaking-points under a progressive ratio
schedule for a sucrose solution reinforcer, and decrements in anticipatory and motivational measures for sexual reinforcement in rats (Barr and Phillips 1999; Barr et al. 1999). Taken
together, these results provide converging evidence that drug withdrawal is characterized by decreased motivation and “diminished interest or pleasure” for a variety of rewarding stimuli and
situations, a condition resembling a core feature of non-drug-induced depressions (i.e., construct validity) (American Psychiatric Association 1994). Although termination of nicotine
administration induced the anticipated somatic signs of withdrawal, none of the drug treatments altered the number of somatic signs observed during nicotine withdrawal. This dissociation of
the effects of the drug combination treatment on the affective (i.e., reward threshold elevations) versus the somatic signs of withdrawal is evidence for symptom-specific neurobiological
abnormalities, and demonstrates the discriminant validity of this animal model (Geyer and Markou 1995). If, as suggested, the affective symptoms of withdrawal are critical to the development
of drug dependence and in causing relapse during the early stages of abstinence (Koob et al. 1993; Markou et al. 1998), then reversal of such symptoms with serotonergic antidepressant
treatments may reduce the probability of relapse. Indeed, there is evidence that antidepressant treatment results in improvement of mood and reduction of drug use in depressed cocaine,
opiate and nicotine abusers (for reviews, see Hughes et al. 2000; Kosten et al. 1998; Markou et al. 1998; Nunes et al. 1998; Watkins et al. 2000a). The administration of fluoxetine together
with a 5-HT1A receptor antagonist also had dissociable effects on the reward function of withdrawing and non-withdrawing rats. The restoration of the sensitivity of the brain reward system
to the electrical stimuli (i.e., relative increase in reward) after the drug treatment in withdrawing rats contrasts with the small decrease in brain stimulation reward induced by the same
drug treatment in saline-treated rats. This result is consistent with previous studies of “normal” unperturbed rats in which increasing serotonergic neurotransmission reduced response rates
for brain stimulation reward reflecting a decrease in reward (McClelland et al. 1989; Olds 1994). These data from “normal” subjects may seem incompatible with the hypothesis that
antidepressants elevate mood and alleviate reward deficits in depressed individuals through enhancement of serotonergic neurotransmission. This apparent disparity may be explained by the
present results suggesting that the effects of antidepressant treatments on brain reward function may be dependent on the “hedonic” state of the subjects at the time of treatment (Ahmed and
Koob 1998; Koob and Le Moal 1997). The present observation also may explain why previous attempts to show antidepressant-induced alterations in “mood” in unperturbed rats have provided
largely negative results (Fibiger and Phillips 1981; Hall et al. 1990; Lin et al. 1999; Moreau et al. 1992; Markou et al. 1992). In the present study and in confirmation of previous work
(Lin et al. 1999), administration of low fluoxetine doses alone did not elevate thresholds, paralleling the lack of effect of antidepressants on mood in healthy individuals. Thus, the
development of animal models of depression, and potentially of other psychiatric disorders, should be based primarily on modeling and studying deficits that are pathognomic of the disorder;
because the predictive validity of models that study healthy “normal” animals may be limited and could lead to erroneous conclusions. In summary, the present results indicated that acute
administration of a drug combination that enhances serotonergic neurotransmission alleviated the diminished interest in brain stimulation reward observed during withdrawal from nicotine or
amphetamine in rats. By contrast, the same drug combination treatment did not reduce the somatic signs of nicotine withdrawal indicating symptom-specific neurobiological abnormalities. Taken
together with extensive previous observations about depression, the present findings suggest that the affective aspects of both drug- and non-drug-induced depressions may be mediated, at
least partly, by reduced serotonergic neurotransmission. Thus, there may be homology between the affective symptoms of drug withdrawal and the core symptom of “diminished interest or
pleasure” characterizing non-drug-induced depressions. Surprisingly, the same serotonergic drug combination treatment had opposite effects in control rats where reductions in reward were
produced. This last observation has important implications about the development of animal models for studying the neurobiology of psychiatric disorders, such as drug withdrawal and
depression, and predicting the clinical efficacy of novel treatments. REFERENCES * Ahmed S, Koob GF . (1998): Transition from moderate to excessive drug intake: Change in hedonic set point.
_Science_ 282: 298–300 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Allen AR, Singh A, Zhuang Z-P, Kung M-P, Kung HF, Lucki I . (1997): The 5-HT1A receptor antagonist p-MPPI blocks responses mediated by
postsynaptic and presynaptic 5-HT1A receptors. _Pharmacol Biochem Behav_ 57: 301–307 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * American Psychiatric Association. (1994): _Diagnostic and Statistical
Manual of Mental Disorders_, 4th Edition. Washington DC, American Psychiatric Press * Artigas F, Romero L, de Montigny C, Blier P . (1996): Acceleration of the effect of selected
antidepressant drugs in major depression by 5-HT1A antagonists. _Trends Neurosci_ 19: 378–383 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Assie MB, Koek W . (1996): (-)-Pindolol and (±)-tertatolol affect
rat hippocampal 5-HT levels through mechanisms involving not only 5-HT1A but also 5-HT1B receptors. _Neuropharmacology_ 35: 213–222 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Auerbach SB, Hjorth S .
(1995): Effect of chronic administration of the selective serotonin (5-HT) uptake inhibitor citalopram on extracellular 5-HT and apparent autoreceptor sensitivity in rat forebrain in vivo.
_Naunyn-Schmiedebergs Arch Pharmacol_ 352: 597–606 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Baldo BA, Koob GF, Markou A . (1999): Role of adenosine a2 receptors in brain stimulation reward under
baseline conditions and during cocaine withdrawal in rats. _J Neurosci_ 19: 11017–11026 CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar * Barr AM, Fiorino DF, Phillips AG . (1999): Effects of
withdrawal from an escalating dose schedule of d-amphetamine on sexual behavior in the male rat. _Pharmacol Biochem Behav_ 64: 597–604 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Barr AM, Phillips AG .
(1999): Withdrawal following repeated exposure to d-amphetamine decreases responding for a sucrose solution as measured by a progressive ratio schedule of reinforcement. _Psychopharmacology_
141: 99–106 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Bauco P, Wise RA . (1994): Potentiation of lateral hypothalamic and midline mesencephalic brain stimulation reinforcement by nicotine: Examination
of repeated treatment. _J Pharmacol Exp Ther_ 271: 294–301 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Bel N, Artigas F . (1993): Chronic treatment with fluvoxamine increases extracellular serotonin in
the frontal cortex but not in raphe nuclei. _Synapse_ 15: 243–245 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Bengtsson HJ, Milano S . (1996): Raphe 5-HT1A autoreceptors, but not postsynaptic 5-HT1A
receptors or beta-adrenoreceptors, restrain the citalopram-induced increase in extracellular 5-hydroxytryptamine in vivo. _Eur J Pharmacol_ 316: 43–47 PubMed Google Scholar * Berman RM,
Anand A, Cappiello A, Miller HL, Hu XS, Oren DA, Charney DS . (1999): The use of pindolol with fluoxetine in the treatment of major depression: Final results from a double-blind,
placebo-controlled trial. _Biol Psychiatry_ 45: 1170–1177 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Berman RM, Darnell AM, Miller HL, Anand A, Charney DS . (1997): Effect of pindolol in hastening
response to fluoxetine in the treatment of major depression: A double-blind, placebo-controlled trial. _Am J Psychiatry_ 154: 37–43 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Blier P, de Montigny C .
(1994): Current advances and trends in the treatment of depression. _Trends Pharmacol Sci_ 15: 220–226 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Bordet R, Thomas P, Dupuis B . (1998): Effect of
pindolol on onset of action of paroxetine in the treatment of major depression: Intermediate analysis of a double-blind, placebo-controlled trial. _Am J Psychiatry_ 155: 1346–1351 CAS
PubMed Google Scholar * Bourin M, Redrobe JP, Baker GB . (1998): Pindolol does not act only on 5-HT1A receptors in augmenting antidepressant activity in the mouse forced swimming test.
_Psychopharmacology_ 136: 226–234 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Caccia S, Cappi M, Fracasso C, Garattinis S . (1990): Influence of dose and route of administration on the kinetics of
fluoxetine and its metabolite norfluoxetine in the rat. _Psychopharmacology_ 100: 509–514 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Caldecott-Hazard S, Morgan DG, DeLeon-Jones M, Overstreet DH,
Janowsky D . (1991): Clinical and biochemical aspects of depressive disorders. II. Transmitter/receptor theories. _Synapse_ 9: 251–301 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Caldecott-Hazard S,
Schneider LS . (1992): Clinical and biochemical aspects of depressive disorders: III. Treatment and controversies. _Synapse_ 10: 141–168 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Carroll ME, Lac ST,
Asencio M, Keenan RM . (1989): Nicotine dependence in rats. _Life Sci_ 45: 1381–1388 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Clarke PBS, Kumar R . (1984): Effects of nicotine and d-amphetamine on
intracranial self-stimulation in a shuttle box test in rats. _Psychopharmacology_ 84: 109–114 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Clifford EM, Gartside SE, Umbers V, Cowen PJ, Hajos M, Sharp T .
(1998): Electrophysiological and neurochemical evidence that pindolol has agonist properties at the 5-HT1A autoreceptor in vivo. _Br J Pharmacol_ 124: 206–212 CAS PubMed PubMed Central
Google Scholar * Covey LS, Glassman AH, Stetner F . (1998): Cigarette smoking and major depression. _J Addict Dis_ 17 (1): 35–46 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Cryan JF, McGrath C, Leonard
BE, Norman TR . (1998): Combining pindolol and paroxetine in an animal model of chronic antidepressant action: Can early onset of action be detected? _Eur J Pharmacol_ 352: 23–28 CAS PubMed
Google Scholar * Cryan JF, McGrath C, Leonard BE, Norman TR . (1999): Onset of the effects of the 5-HT1A antagonist, WAY-100635, alone, and in combination with paroxetine, on olfactory
bulbectomy and 8-OH-DPAT-induced changes in the rat. _Pharmacol Biochem Behav_ 63: 333–338 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Detke MJ, Johnson J, Lucki I . (1997): Acute and chronic
antidepressant drug treatment in the rat forced swimming test model of depression. _Exp Clin Psychopharmacol_ 5 (2): 107–112 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Dreshfield LJ, Rocco VP, Wong DT .
(1997): Greater effects of fluoxetine and its combination with (-)-pindolol in elevating hypothalamic serotonin in rats during dark hours. _Chinese J Physiol_ 40 (2): 57–61 CAS Google
Scholar * Dreshfield LJ, Wong DT, Perry KW, Engleman EA . (1996): Enhancement of fluoxetine-dependent increase of extracellular serotonin (5-HT) levels by (-)-pindolol, an antagonist at
5-HT1A receptors. _Neurochem Res_ 21: 557–562 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Epping-Jordan MP, Watkins SS, Koob GF, Markou A . (1998): Dramatic decreases in brain reward function during
nicotine withdrawal. _Nature_ 393: 76–79 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Fibiger HC, Phillips AG . (1981): Increased intracranial self-stimulation in rats after long-term administration of
desipramine. _Science_ 214: 683–685 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Fornal CA, Martin FJ, Mendlin A, Metzler CW, Bjorvatn B, Jacobs BL . (1999a): Pindolol increases extracellular 5-HT while
inhibiting serotonergic neuronal activity. _Eur J Pharmacol_ 377: 187–191 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Fornal CA, Martin FJ, Metzler CW, Jacobs BL . (1999b): Pindolol, a putative
5-hydroxytryptamine1A antagonist, does not reverse the inhibition of serotonergic neuronal activity induced by fluoxetine in awake cats: Comparison to WAY-100635. _J Pharmacol Exp Ther_ 291
(1): 220–228 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Fornal CA, Martin FJ, Metzler CW, Jacobs BL . (1999c): Pindolol suppresses serotonergic neuronal activity and does not block the inhibition of
serotonergic neurons produced by 8-hydroxy-2-(di-n-propylamino)tetralin in awake cats. _J Pharmacol Exp Ther_ 291: 229–238 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Frazer A . (1994): Antidepressant
drugs. _Depression_ 2: 1–19 Google Scholar * Geyer MA, Markou A . (1995): Animal models of psychiatric disorders. In Bloom FE, Kupfer DJ (eds), _Psychopharmacology: The Fourth Generation of
Progress._ New York, Raven Press, pp 787–798 Google Scholar * Glassman AH . (1993): Cigarette-smoking: implications for psychiatric illness. _Am J Psychiatry_ 150: 546–553 CAS PubMed
Google Scholar * Gobert A, Millan MJ . (1999): Modulation of dialysate levels of dopamine, noradrenaline, and serotonin (5-HT) in the frontal cortex of freely-moving rats by (-)-pindolol
alone and in association with 5-HT reuptake inhibitors: Comparative roles of α-adrenergic, 5-HT1A, and 5-HT1B receptors. _Neuropsychopharmacology_ 21: 268–284 CAS PubMed Google Scholar *
Grunberg NE, Bowen DJ, Winders SE . (1986): Effects of nicotine on body weight and food consumption in female rats. _Psychopharmacology_ 90: 101–105 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Hall FS,
Stellar JR, Kelley AE . (1990): Acute and chronic desipramine treatment effects on rewarding electrical stimulation of the lateral hypothalamus. _Pharmacol Biochem Behav_ 37: 277–281 CAS
PubMed Google Scholar * Harrison AA, Parsons LH, Koob GF, Markou A . (1999): RU 24969, a 5-HT1A/1B agonist, elevates brain stimulation reward thresholds: An effect reversed by GR127935, a
selective 5-HT 1B/1D antagonist. _Psychopharmacology_ 141: 242–250 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Heninger GR, Delgado PL, Charney DS . (1996): The revised monoamine theory of depression: A
modulatory role for monoamines, based on new findings from monoamine depletion experiments in humans. _Pharmacopsychiatry_ 29: 2–11 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Hildebrand BE, Nomikos GG,
Bondjers C, Nisell M, Svensson TH . (1997): Behavioral manifestations of the nicotine abstinence syndrome in the rat: peripheral versus central mechanisms. _Psychopharmacology_ 129: 348–356
CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Hildebrand BE, Panagis G, Svensson TH, Nomikos GG . (1999): Behavioral and biochemical manifestations of mecamylamine-precipitated nicotine withdrawal in the
rat: Role of nicotinic receptors in the ventral tegmental area. _Neuropsychopharmacology_ 21: 560–574 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Hjorth S . (1993): Serotonin 5-HT1A autoreceptor blockade
potentiates the ability of the 5-HT reuptake inhibitor citalopram to increase nerve terminal output of 5-HT in vivo: A microdialysis study. _J Neurochem_ 60: 776–779 CAS PubMed Google
Scholar * Hjorth S . (1996): (-)-Pindolol, but not buspirone, potentiates the citalopram-induced rise in extracellular 5-hydroxytryptamine. _Eur J Pharmacol_ 303: 183–186 CAS PubMed
Google Scholar * Hoyer D, Schoeffter P . (1991): 5-HT receptors: Subtypes and second messengers. _J Recept Res_ 11: 197–214 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Hughes JR . (1992): Tobacco
withdrawal in self-quitters. _J Consult Clin Psychol_ 60: 689–697 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Hughes JR, Stead LF, Lancaster T . (2000): Anxiolytics and antidepressants for smoking
cessation. _Cochrane Database of Systemic Reviews 2: CD000031_ * Huston-Lyons D, Kornetsky C . (1992): Effects of nicotine on the threshold for rewarding brain stimulation in rats.
_Pharmacol Biochem Behav_ 41: 755–759 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Invernizzi R, Bramante M, Samanin R . (1994): Chronic treatment with citalopram facilitates the effect of a challenge
dose on cortical serotonin output: Role of presynaptic 5-HT1A receptors. _Eur J Pharmacol_ 260: 243–246 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Knobelman DA, Kung HF, Lucki I . (2000): Regulation of
extracellular concentrations of 5-hydroxytryptamine (5-HT) in mouse striatum by 5-HT(1A) and 5-HT(1B) receptors. _J Pharmacol Exp Ther_ 292 (3): 1111–1117 CAS PubMed Google Scholar *
Kokkinidis L, McCarter BD . (1990): Postcocaine depression and sensitization of brain stimulation reward: Analysis of reinforcement and performance effects. _Pharmacol Biochem Behav_ 36:
463–471 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Kokkinidis L, Zacharko RM . (1980a): Enhanced lateral hypothalamic self-stimulation responding after chronic exposure to amphetamine. _Behav Neural
Biol_ 29: 493–497 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Kokkinidis L, Zacharko RM . (1980b): Response sensitization and depression following long-term amphetamine treatment in a self-stimulation
paradigm. _Psychopharmacology_ 68: 73–76 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Kokkinidis L, Zacharko RM, Anisman H . (1986): Amphetamine withdrawal: A behavioral evaluation. _Life Sci_ 38:
1617–1623 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Kokkinidis L, Zacharko RM, Predy PA . (1980): Post-amphetamine depression of self-stimulation responding from the substantia nigra: Reversal by
tricyclic antidepressants. _Pharmacol Biochem Behav_ 13: 379–383 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Koob GF, Le Moal M . (1997): Drug abuse: Hedonic homeostatic dysregulation. _Science_ 278:
52–58. CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Koob GF, Markou A, Weiss F, Schulteis G . (1993): Opponent process and drug dependence. _Seminars Neurosci_ 5: 351–358. Google Scholar * Kornetsky C,
Esposito RU . (1979): Euphorigenic drugs: Effects on the reward pathways of the brain. _Federation Proc_ 38: 2473–2476. CAS Google Scholar * Kosten TR, Markou A, Koob GF . (1998):
Depression and stimulant dependence: Neurobiology and pharmacotherapy. _J Nerv Ment Dis_ 186: 737–745 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Kreiss DS, Lucki I . (1994): Differential regulation of
serotonin (5-HT) release in the striatum and hippocampus by 5-HT1A autoreceptors of the dorsal and median raphe nuclei. _J Pharmacol Exp Ther_ 269: 1268–1279 CAS PubMed Google Scholar *
Kreiss DS, Lucki I . (1995): Effects of acute and repeated administration of antidepressant drugs on extracellular levels of 5-hydroxytryptamine measured in vivo. _J Pharmacol Exp Ther_ 274:
866–876 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Kreiss DS, Lucki I . (1997): Chronic administration of the 5-HT1A receptor agonist 8-OH-DPAT differentially desensitizes 5-HT1A autoreceptors of the
dorsal and median raphe nuclei. _Synapse_ 25 (2): 107–116 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Kung HF, Kung M-P, Clarke W, Maayani S, Zhuang Z-P . (1994a): A potential 5-HT1A receptor antagonist:
p-MPPI. _Life Sci_ 55: 1459–1462 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Kung M-P, Frederick D, Mu M, Zhuang Z-P, Kung HF . (1995):
4-(2′-methoxy-phenyl)-1-(2′-(N-2″-pyridinyl)-p-iodobenzamido]-ethyl-piperazine ([125I]p-MPPI) as a new selective radioligand of serotonin-1A sites in rat brain: In vitro binding and
autoradiographic studies. _J Pharmacol Exp Ther_ 272: 429–437 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Kung M-P, Zhuang Z-P, Frederick D, Kung HF . (1994b): In vivo binding of
[123I]4-(2′-methoxy-phenyl)-1-(2′-(N-2″-pyridinyl)-p-iodobenzamido-]ethyl-piperazine, p-MPPI, to 5-HT1A receptors in rat brain. _Synapse_ 18: 359–366 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Lee K,
Kornetsky C . (1998): Acute and chronic fluoxetine treatment decreases the sensitivity of rats to rewarding brain stimulation. _Pharmacol Biochem Behav_ 60: 539–544 CAS PubMed Google
Scholar * Leith NJ, Barrett RJ . (1976): Amphetamine and the reward system: Evidence for tolerance and post-drug depression. _Psychopharmacologica_ 46: 19–25 CAS Google Scholar * Leith
NJ, Barrett RJ . (1980): Effects of chronic amphetamine or reserpine on self-stimulation responding: Animal model of depression? _Psychopharmacology_ 72: 9–15 CAS PubMed Google Scholar *
Levin ED, Morgan MM, Galvez C, Ellison GD . (1987): Chronic nicotine and withdrawal effects on body weight and food and water consumption in female rats. _Physiol Behav_ 39: 441–444 CAS
PubMed Google Scholar * Lin D, Koob GF, Markou A . (1999): Differential effects of withdrawal from chronic amphetamine or fluoxetine administration on brain reward in the rat: Interactions
between the two drugs. _Psychopharmacology_ 145: 283–294 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Lin D, Koob GF, Markou A . (2000): Time-dependent alterations in ICSS thresholds associated with
repeated amphetamine administrations. _Pharmacol Biochem Behav_ 65: 407–417 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Lucki I . (1997): The forced swimming test as a model for core and component
behavioral effects of antidepresssant drugs. _Behav Pharmacol_ 8: 523–532 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Macey DJ, Koob GF, Markou A . (2000): CRF and urocortin decreased brain stimulation
reward in the rat: Reversal by a CRF receptor antagonist. _Brain Res_ 866: 82–91 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Maes M, Libbrecht I, van Hunsel F, Campens D, Meltzer HY . (1999): Pindolol
and mianserin augment the antidepressant activity of fluoxetine in hospitalized major depressed patients, including those with treatment resistance. _J Clin Psychopharmacol_ 19: 177–182 CAS
PubMed Google Scholar * Malin DH, Lake JR, Newlin-Maultsby P, Roberts LK, Lanier JG, Carter VA, Cunningham JS, Wilson OB . (1992): Rodent model of nicotine abstinence syndrome.
_Pharmacol Biochem Behav_ 43: 779–784 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Markou A, Hauger RL, Koob GF . (1992): Desmethylimipramine attenuates cocaine withdrawal in rats. _Psychopharmacology_
109: 305–314 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Markou A, Koob GF . (1991): Postcocaine anhedonia: An animal model of cocaine withdrawal. _Neuropsychopharmacology_ 4: 17–26 CAS PubMed Google
Scholar * Markou A, Koob GF . (1992a): Bromocriptine reverses the elevation in intracranial self-stimulation thresholds observed in a rat model of cocaine withdrawal.
_Neuropsychopharmacolgy_ 7: 213–224 CAS Google Scholar * Markou A, Koob GF . (1992b): Construct validity of a self-stimulation threshold paradigm: Effects of reward and performance
manipulations. _Physiol Behav_ 51: 111–119 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Markou A, Koob GF . (1993): Intracranial self-stimulation thresholds as a measure of reward. In Sahgal AB (ed),
_Behavioural Neuroscience Volume II: A Practical Approach._ New York, Oxford University Press, pp 93–115 Google Scholar * Markou A, Kosten TR, Koob GF . (1998): Neurobiological similarities
in depression and drug dependence: A self medication hypothesis. _Neuropsychopharmacology_ 18: 135–174 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Marubio LM, del Mar Arroyo-Jimenez M, Cordero-Erausquin
M, Lena C, Le Novere N, de Kerchove d'Exaerde A, Huchet M, Damaj MI, Changeux J-P . (1999): Reduced antinociception in mice lacking neuronal nicotinic receptor subunits. _Nature_ 398:
805–810 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * McAskill R, Mir S, Taylor D . (1998): Pindolol augmentation of antidepressant therapy. _Br J Psychiatry_ 173: 203–208 CAS PubMed Google Scholar *
McClelland RC, Sarfaty T, Hernandez L, Hoebel BG . (1989): The appetite suppressant, d-fenfluramine, decreases self-stimulation at a feeding site in the lateral hypothalamus. _Pharmacol
Biochem Behav_ 32: 411–414 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Meltzer HY, Lowy MT . (1988): The serotonin hypothesis of depression. In Meltzer HY (ed), _Psychopharmacology: The Third Generation
of Progress._ New York, Raven Press, pp 513–526 Google Scholar * Moreau J-L, Jenck F, Martin JR, Mortas P, Haefely WE . (1992): Antidepressant treatment prevents chronic unpredictable mild
stress-induced anhedonia as assessed by ventral tegmentum self-stimulation behavior in rats. _Eur Neuropsychopharmacology_ 2: 43–49 CAS Google Scholar * Moser PC, Sanger DJ . (1999):
5-HT1A receptor antagonists neither potentiate nor inhibit the effects of fluoxetine and befloxatone in the forced swim test in rats. _Eur J Pharmacol_ 372: 127–134 CAS PubMed Google
Scholar * Mousa SA, Aloyo VJ, Van Loon GR . (1988): Tolerance to tobacco smoke- and nicotine-induced analgesia in rats. _Pharmacol Biochem Behav_ 31: 265–268 CAS PubMed Google Scholar *
Newman-Tancredi A, Chaput C, Gavaudan S, Verriele L, Millan MJ . (1998): Agonist and antagonist actions of (-)pindolol at recombinant, human 5-HT1A receptors. _Neuropsychopharmacology_ 18:
395–398 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Nunes EV, Quitkin FM, Donovan SJ, Deliyannides D, Ocepek-Welikson K, Koenig T, Brady R, McGrath PJ, Woody G . (1998): Imipramine treatment of
opiate-dependent patients with depressive disorders: A placebo-controlled trial. _Arch Gen Psychiatry_ 55: 153–160 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Olds ME . (1994): Opposite effects of PCA
and chlorimipramine on ICSS and on its facilitation by amphetamine. _Pharmacol Biochem Behav_ 47: 803–817 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Parsons LH, Koob GF, Weiss F . (1995): Serotonin
dysfunction in the nucleus accumbens of rats during withdrawal after unlimited access to intravenous cocaine. _J Pharmacol Exp Ther_ 274: 1182–1191 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Paterson
NE, Myers C, Markou A . (2000): Effects of repeated withdrawal from continuous amphetamine administration on brain reward function. _Psychopharmacology_ 152: 440–446 CAS PubMed Google
Scholar * Pauwels PJ, Palmier C . (1994a): Differential functional activity of 5-hydroxytryptamine receptor ligands and _beta_ adrenergic receptor antagonists at 5-hydroxytryptamine1B
receptor sites in chinese haster lung fibroblasts and opossum renal epithelial cells. _J Pharmacol Exp Ther_ 270: 938–945 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Pauwels PJ, Palmier C . (1994b):
Inhibition by 5-HT of forskolin-induced cAMP formation in the renal opossum epithelial cell line OK: Mediation by a 5-HT1B like receptor and antagonism by methiothepin. _Neuropharmacology_
33: 67–75 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Pellegrino LJ, Pellegrino AS, Cushman AJ . (1979): _A Stereotaxic Atlas of the Rat Brain_, 2nd ed. New York, Plenum Press Google Scholar * Perez V,
Gilaberte I, Faries D, Alvarez E, Artigas F . (1997): Randomized, double-blind placebo-controlled trial of pindolol in combination with fluoxetine antidepressant treatment. _Lancet_ 349:
1594–1597 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Redrobe JP, Bourin M . (1998): Dose-dependent influence of buspirone on the activities of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors in the mouse forced
swimming test. _Psychopharmacology_ 138: 198–206 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Russell MAH . (1989): Subjective and behavioural effects of nicotine in humans: Some sources of individual
variation. _Prog Brain Res_ 79: 289–302 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Rutter JJ, Gundlah C, Auerbach SB . (1994): Increase in extracellular serotonin produced by uptake inhibitors is
enhanced after chronic treatment with fluoxetine. _Neurosci Lett_ 171: 183–186 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Schulteis G, Markou A, Cole M, Koob GF . (1995): Decreased brain reward produced
by ethanol withdrawal. _Proc Natl Acad Sci USA_ 92: 5880–5884 CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar * Schulteis G, Markou A, Gold LH, Stinus L, Koob GF . (1994): Relative sensitivity
to naloxone of multiple indices of opiate withdrawal: A quantitative dose-response analysis. _J Pharmacol Exp Ther_ 271: 1391–1398 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Swerdlow NR, Geyer MA .
(1998): Using an animal model of sensorimotor gating to study the pathophysiology and new treatments of schizophrenia. _Schizophrenia Bulletin_ 24: 285–301 CAS PubMed Google Scholar *
Tome MB, Cloninger CR, Watson JP, Isaac MT . (1997a): Serotonergic autoreceptor blockade in the reduction of antidepressant latency: Personality variables and response to paroxetine and
pindolol. _J Affect Disord_ 44: 101–109 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Tome MB, Isaac MT . (1998): One year real world prospective follow-up study of a major depressive episode of patients
treated with paroxetine and pindolol or paroxetine for 6 weeks. _Int Clin Psychopharmacol_ 13: 169–174 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Tome MB, Isaac MT, Harte R, Holland C . (1997b):
Paroxetine and pindolol: A randomized trial of serotonergic autoreceptor blockade in the reduction of antidepressant latency. _Int Clin Psychopharmacol_ 12: 81–89 CAS PubMed Google Scholar
* Watkins SS, Koob GF, Markou A . (2000a): Neural mechanisms underlying nicotine addiction: acute positive reinforcement and withdrawal. _Nicotine Tobacco Res_ 2: 19–37 CAS Google Scholar
* Watkins SS, Stinus L, Koob GF, Markou A . (2000b): Reward and somatic changes during precipitated nicotine withdrawal in rats: Centrally and peripherally medicated effects. _J Pharmacol
Exp Ther_ 292: 1053–1064 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Weiss F, Parsons LH, Schulteis G, Hyytia P, Lorang MT, Bloom FE, Koob GF . (1996): Ethanol self-administration restores
withdrawal-associated deficiencies in accumbal dopamine and 5-hydroxytryptamine release in dependent rats. _J Neurosci_ 16: 3474–3485 CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar * Willner P
. (1985): Antidepressants and serotonergic neurotransmission: An integrative review. _Psychopharmacology_ 85: 387–404 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Winders SE, Grunberg NE . (1989):
Nicotine, tobacco smoke, and body weight: A review of the animal literature. _Ann Behav Med_ 11: 125–133 Google Scholar * Wise RA, Munn E . (1995): Withdrawal from chronic amphetamine
elevates baseline intracranial self-stimulation thresholds. _Psychopharmacology_ 117: 130–136 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Wong DT, Bymaster FP, Engleman EA . (1995): Prozac (fluoxetine,
Lilly 110140), the first selective serotonin uptake inhibitor and an antidepressant drug: Twenty years since its first publication. _Life Sci_ 57: 411–441 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Yang
C-Y, Wu W-H, Zbuzek VK . (1992): Antinociceptive effect of chronic nicotine and nociceptive effect of its withdrawal measured by hot-plate and tail-flick in rats. _Psychopharmacology_ 106:
417–420 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Zanardi R, Artigas F, Fanchini L, Sforzini L, Gasperini M, Smeraldi E, Perez J . (1997): How long should pindolol be associated with paroxetine to
improve antidepressant response? _J Clin Psychopharmacol_ 17: 446–450 CAS PubMed Google Scholar * Zanardi R, Franchini L, Gasperini M, Lucca A, Smeraldi E, Perez J . (1998): Faster onset
of fluvoxamine in combination with pindolol in the treatment of delusional depression: A controlled study. _J Clin Psychopharmacol_ 18: 441–446 CAS PubMed Google Scholar Download
references ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This work was supported by a Novartis Research Grant, a Tobacco-Related Disease Research Program Grant from the State of California (7RT-0004), and a National
Institute on Drug Abuse grant (DA 11946) to A.M. We thank Drs. Mark A. Geyer, George F. Koob, Daniel Hoyer and Neil Paterson for their helpful comments and suggestions, and Mike Arends for
computer and library searches, and editorial assistance. This is publication 12431-NP from The Scripps Research Institute. AUTHOR INFORMATION Author notes * Amanda A Harrison Ph.D Present
address: School of Psychology, University of Leeds, Leeds, England, UK AUTHORS AND AFFILIATIONS * Department of Neuropharmacology, Scripps Research Institute, La Jolla, CA, USA Amanda A
Harrison Ph.D & Yves T B Liem Pharm.D * Department of Psychopharmacology, Faculty of Pharmacy, Utrecht University, Sorbonnelaan 16, 3508 TB Utrecht, 3508 TB Utrecht, The Netherlands Yves
T B Liem Pharm.D & Athina Markou Ph.D Authors * Amanda A Harrison Ph.D View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar * Yves T B Liem Pharm.D View
author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar * Athina Markou Ph.D View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar
CORRESPONDING AUTHOR Correspondence to Athina Markou Ph.D. RIGHTS AND PERMISSIONS Reprints and permissions ABOUT THIS ARTICLE CITE THIS ARTICLE Harrison, A., Liem, Y. & Markou, A.
Fluoxetine Combined with a Serotonin-1A Receptor Antagonist Reversed Reward Deficits Observed during Nicotine and Amphetamine Withdrawal in Rats. _Neuropsychopharmacol_ 25, 55–71 (2001).
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0893-133X(00)00237-2 Download citation * Received: 23 June 2000 * Revised: 25 October 2000 * Accepted: 26 October 2000 * Issue Date: 01 July 2001 * DOI:
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0893-133X(00)00237-2 SHARE THIS ARTICLE Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content: Get shareable link Sorry, a shareable link is not
currently available for this article. Copy to clipboard Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative KEYWORDS * Drug withdrawal * Depression * Serotonin * Nicotine *
Amphetamine * Intracranial self-stimulation * Reward * Motivation * Fluoxetine * 5-HT1A antagonist * Somatic signs * Body weight