Play all audios:
Download PDF Article Open access Published: 31 August 2017 Spatial complexity of carcass location influences vertebrate scavenger efficiency and species composition Joshua B. Smith1, Lauren
J. Laatsch2 & James C. Beasley2 Scientific Reports volume 7, Article number: 10250 (2017) Cite this article
2828 Accesses
Metrics details
Subjects Ecosystem servicesFood webs AbstractScavenging plays an important role in shaping communities through inter- and intra-specific interactions. Although vertebrate scavenger efficiency and species composition is likely
influenced by the spatial complexity of environments, heterogeneity in carrion distribution has largely been disregarded in scavenging studies. We tested this hypothesis by experimentally
placing juvenile bird carcasses on the ground and in nests in trees to simulate scenarios of nestling bird carrion availability. We used cameras to record scavengers removing carcasses and
elapsed time to removal. Carrion placed on the ground was scavenged by a greater diversity of vertebrates and at > 2 times the rate of arboreal carcasses, suggesting arboreal carrion may
represent an important resource to invertebrate scavengers, particularly in landscapes with efficient vertebrate scavenging communities. Nonetheless, six vertebrate species scavenged
arboreal carcasses. Rat snakes (Elaphe obsolete), which exclusively scavenged from trees, and turkey vultures (Cathartes aura) were the primary scavengers of arboreal carrion, suggesting
such resources are potentially an important pathway of nutrient acquisition for some volant and scansorial vertebrates. Our results highlight the intricacy of carrion-derived food web
linkages, and how consideration of spatial complexity in carcass distribution (i.e., arboreal) may reveal important pathways of nutrient acquisition by invertebrate and vertebrate scavenging
guilds.
Similar content being viewed by others Carcass detection and consumption by facultative scavengers in forest ecosystem highlights the value of their ecosystem services ArticleOpen access 30 September 2022 Factors influencing scavenger guilds and scavenging efficiency in Southwestern Montana Article Open access 19 February 2021 Influence of heterospecifics on
mesocarnivore behaviour at shared scavenging opportunities in the Canadian Rocky Mountains Article Open access 07 July 2023 Introduction
Research suggests a substantial number of animals die from causes other than predation, such as disease, and their carcasses become temporary food resources for a diversity of scavengers1,
2. Consequently, scavenging is an important mechanism facilitating energy flow throughout food webs that encompasses a wide range of organisms across multiple kingdoms3,4,5. Although
carcasses are used extensively by invertebrates and microbes6, vertebrates often consume a substantive proportion of available carcasses7, 8, which varies spatially and temporally as a
function of biotic and abiotic factors including temperature, habitat, carcass size, and presence of humans3, 9,10,11,12. Additionally, scavenging plays an important role in shaping
communities through inter- and intra-specific interactions because the presence of carrion provides a temporary resource over which vertebrates, invertebrates, and microbes compete2, 8, 13,
14. Recent studies have shed light on the ecological4, 5, 13, 15 and economic16 importance of vertebrate scavengers, yet we are only beginning to understand how decomposition and scavenging
dynamics differ within three-dimensional space9, 14.
In particular, spatial heterogeneity in the distribution of carrion (e.g., below ground, arboreal, under leaf litter, terrestrial surface) has largely been disregarded in scavenging studies,
yet likely has profound effects on carcass fate, persistence times, and scavenger interactions9. Indeed, previous research shows microsite differences (e.g. upland vs. lowland habitats,
open fields vs. burrows) can affect competition for carrion, mammalian contact rates – which could increase disease transmission, and decomposition times11, 17, 18. Although carrion
predominantly occurs on the surface in terrestrial environments19, subterranean (e.g., carcasses in burrows), subsurface (e.g., rodents or caching of prey by predators), and arboreal
carcasses also represent potentially important pathways of carrion availability19,20,21,22. However, we are not aware of any research that has evaluated the fate of arboreal carrion as most
scavenging studies have experimentally placed carcasses on the ground. Such alternative forms of carrion availability, despite their reduced availability compared to surface carrion, could
represent important pathways of nutrient acquisition by scavenging guilds often outcompeted for carrion resources, especially for invertebrates in landscapes with high densities of
vertebrate scavengers able to consume 90% or more of available surface carrion7. For example, access to arboreal carrion is limited to volant or scansorial species and thus nutrients
associated with such carcasses are likely available to a restricted suite of scavengers (both vertebrates and invertebrates) compared to surface carrion, potentially resulting in reduced
scavenging efficiency by vertebrates. Moreover, disregarding such environmental heterogeneity is likely to result in a simplified understanding of functional redundancy within the scavenging
community (e.g. ref. 23), and fails to account for potential emerging scavengers in novel environments.
Furthermore, despite the abundance and global distribution of birds and the fact that nesting mortality rates may be high20, 24, few studies have investigated the fate of avian
carrion25,26,27 and no studies to date have examined the fate of juvenile bird carcasses. There are several causes of nestling mortality including insufficient parental care28, siblicide29,
starvation29, disease30, extreme weather events, and abandonment20. All of these causes of death create carrion resources for scavengers. Nestling carcasses may be left in the nest, such as
in the case of abandonment, or pushed out of the nest onto the ground, as in cases of siblicide. Although birds likely comprise a substantial proportion of arboreal carrion, arboreal
carcasses are not only limited to birds as some reptiles (e.g., snakes, lizards) and mammals (e.g., squirrels, raccoons – Procyon lotor) den or concentrate activities in trees.
Given the lack of knowledge regarding the fate of arboreal carcasses, coupled with the fact that other forms of spatial heterogeneity in carrion availability contribute to considerable
variability in scavenging community dynamics and thus nutrient cycling1, 9, 10, further research on the fate of non-surface carrion is needed to better elucidate food web dynamics in
terrestrial ecosystems. Furthermore, data on scavenging of nestling bird carcasses will allow for a more comprehensive understanding of the ecological consequences of bird declines or
implications of die-offs in terms of energy flow and transfer of toxicants31. To address knowledge gaps regarding the fate of nestling bird carcasses, as well as potential differences in the
composition of scavenging communities between arboreal and ground-based carrion, our goal in this study was to test the hypotheses that (1) arboreal carrion represents a potentially
important source of nutrients for scavenging communities, but that (2) scavenging rates by vertebrate scavengers and (3) vertebrate scavenger guilds differ between arboreal and terrestrial
carrion. We predicted arboreal-placed carcasses would persist longer and be utilized by a smaller suite of vertebrate scavengers, thus potentially serving as critical resources for
invertebrates, and even some specialized vertebrates, especially in areas with highly efficient vertebrate scavenging communities. This research provides novel evidence of scavenging ecology
in a previously undocumented environment and allows us to predict how carcass locations influence nutrient recycling in forested ecosystems.
ResultsWe conducted 214 scavenging trials over two summers, 120 trials between May and July 2015 and 94 from June to July 2016. We discarded one quail carcass placed in a tree nest due to the
carcass blowing out of nest <3 hrs after placement. Overall, 16.4% (n = 35) of carcasses were scavenged by vertebrates and 4.2% (n = 9) had an unknown fate (Table 1). Of 107 carcasses placed
in trees, 11 (10.3%) were scavenged by vertebrates compared to 24 (22.6%) of 106 carcasses placed on the ground (Table 1). Overall 16 (15.1%) quail and 19 (17.8%) chicken carcasses were
scavenged (Table 1).
Table 1 Fate of 210 nestling chicken and quail carcasses that were monitored by remote cameras based on scavenging trials conducted 29 May to 29 July 2015 and 30 Juneto 27 July 2016 in forested plots at the Savannah River Site, Aiken, SC, USA.Full size table
Average persistence time was 38.7 hrs (SE = 2.0 hrs), although we observed a high degree of variability in decomposition time (Fig. 1). Carcasses placed in tree nests tended to remain
available to potential scavengers longer than those on the ground, and in the absence of vertebrate scavengers, 50% of all carcasses were decomposed by invertebrates and microbes within 39
hrs (range 32 hrs for chicken carcasses placed in trees to 39 hrs for chicken placed on the ground and quail in trees; Fig. 1). Results of our log-rank test indicated significant differences
(X 2 3 = 10.3, P = 0.017) in persistence times between carcass sizes and habitat types. This was primarily due to quail carcasses placed on the ground persisting an average of 26.8 hrs (SE
= 2.4 hrs) versus >42.3 hrs (SE = 2.4), on average, for quail placed in trees and for chicken carcasses in both habitat types (Fig. 2).
Figure 1Probability of quail and chicken carcasses placed in trees and on the ground persisting on the landscape based on scavenging trials conducted 29 May to 29 July 2015 and 30 June to 27 July
2016 in forested plots at the Savannah River Site, Aiken, SC, USA. All carcasses scavenged by vertebrates were censored at time taken.
Full size image Figure 2Average persistence time (i.e., decomposing or taken by invertebrates) with confidence intervals of quail and chicken carcasses placed in trees and on the ground (carcasses taken by
vertebrates were censored at time of take) based on scavenging trials conducted 29 May to 29 July 2015 and 30 June to 27 July 2016 in forested plots at the Savannah River Site, Aiken, SC,
USA.
Full size imageAmong carcasses scavenged by vertebrates, overall average time until removal was 34.2 hrs (SE = 4.8 hrs), and ranged from 33.9 hrs (SE = 6.1 hrs) for those on the ground to 34.8 hrs (SE =
7.4 hrs) for carcasses in tree nests. Average time to removal of chicken carcasses was 44.0 hrs (SE = 7.3 hrs), compared to 22.5 hrs (SE = 4.5 hrs) for quail carcasses. Around the average
removal times carcasses tended to be intact and exhibiting slight degradation from invertebrate activity. From the 35 positively identified vertebrate scavenger trials, model results
constructed to determine differences in length of time until removal indicated the model containing only species was our best approximating model (w i = 0.63). Remaining models were ≥2.0
∆AICc units from this model, and the weight of evidence supporting the top model was >1.7 times greater than all other models combined (see Supplementary Table S1). The β estimate for
species was −0.690 (CI = −1.19 – −0.19) indicating quail carcasses were removed by vertebrates more quickly than chicken carcasses.
In contrast, linear mixed effect models constructed to examine difference in carcass fate (i.e., scavenged versus not scavenged by a vertebrate) indicated our most supported model was one
that contained only habitat (see Supplementary Table S2). While one additional model fell within ≤2.0 ∆AICc (model {habitat + species}), the β estimate for covariate species = 0 (CI =
−0.97–0.54), thus, we considered this an uninformative variable32. The β estimate from our top model {habitat} was −1.15 (CI = −1.88 – −0.29) indicating carcasses placed in trees were less
likely to be scavenged than those on the ground (Table S2).
Ten vertebrate species were documented scavenging ≥1 carcass during our trials, with two additional unknowns and one snake not identifiable to species. Of the 10 identifiable species, nine
(90%) were observed scavenging ground-based carrion, while six (60%) were documented scavenging from trees (Table 2). Five species removed carcasses from both habitat types with rat snakes
(Elaphe obsolete) representing the only species that scavenged exclusively from trees (Table 2). Overall, snakes were the most frequently observed scavenger taxa accounting for 14 (40.0%) of
all scavenging events, and were represented by three different species; copperhead (Agkistrodon contortrix, n = 2), rat snake (n = 4), and black racer (Coluber constrictor, n = 7). Turkey
vultures (Cathartes aura) accounted for eight (22.9%) scavenging events and raccoons (Procyon lotor) three (8.6%). Coyote (Canis latrans), gray fox (Urocyon cinereoargenteus), wild pigs (Sus
scrofa), and Virginia opossum (Didelphis virginana) each accounted for two (5.7%) events, while an eastern box turtle (Terrapene carolina) was observed scavenging one carcass (2.8%; Table
2).
Table 2 Scavenger species take of experimentally-placed carcasses by habitat and carcass type based on scavenging trials conducted 29 May to 29 July 2015 and 30 June to 27 July 2016in forested plots at the Savannah River Site, Aiken, SC, USA.Full size table
Percent occurrence of several scavenger species varied noticeably by habitat and carcass type (Fig. 3). Rat snakes exhibited the highest value along dimension 2, primarily due to all
scavenging events for this species occurring in trees. In contrast, wild pigs and box turtles only scavenged ground carrion and exhibited the lowest value along dimension 2. Differences in
dimension 1 were primarily driven by carcass type. The greatest dichotomy occurred between coyote/opossum and copperheads, due to their association with chicken and quail carcasses,
respectively. Fox and raccoons were the least discriminatory for either carcass or habitat type, while black racer’s tended to take both carcass types from the ground.
Figure 3Figure shows scavenger species (blue dots), habitat and carcass type (red arrows) projected in top two major correspondence analysis dimensions with variance shown in parentheses based on
scavenging trials conducted 29 May to 29 July 2015 and 30 June to 27 July 2016 in forested plots at the Savannah River Site, Aiken, SC, USA.
Full size image DiscussionOur study suggests arboreal carrion is potentially an important pathway of nutrient acquisition by some volant and scansorial vertebrates, especially rat snakes specialized in arboreal
foraging. Nonetheless, terrestrial carcasses were scavenged at more than double the rate of arboreal carcasses and thus more energy is likely transferred from terrestrial carcasses to
vertebrate scavengers than from arboreal carcasses. This suggests arboreal carrion may represent an important resource to invertebrate scavengers, particularly in landscapes where
vertebrates consume nearly all available terrestrial carrion (e.g. ref. 7). The difference in scavenging rates observed among habitats was likely because terrestrial carcasses were available
to a wider range of vertebrate scavenger species. In fact, of the ten vertebrate species we recorded scavenging, only rat snakes scavenged arboreal carcasses exclusively. The nests in our
study were placed on relatively low branches of trees but had arboreal carcasses been higher up, the number of scavenged carcasses would likely have been lower. Several studies have shown
mammalian predators more frequently depredate avian nests located lower to the ground, and nest success rates increase when nests are higher33, 34. In fact, Fisher and Wiebe33 found that,
despite a difference of approximately 0.5 m, higher nests were more successful. Such small variations likely act within scavenging guilds as well, with arboreal carrion releasing some of the
competitive pressures that exists for ground-based carrion. The added complexity of these environments could, however, expose new scavenger guilds in comparison to more competitive
landscapes and function to reduce interspecific competition in comparison to less heterogeneous landscapes.
Carcasses that were not scavenged by vertebrates were unlikely to persist more than 48 hours, illustrating the efficiency at which invertebrates and microbes remove these items. Despite this
relatively rapid removal, vertebrates managed to scavenge more than 16% of all carcasses, similar rates as those reported in the summer by DeVault et al.1 in 2004 –19% – for
experimentally-placed mouse (Mus musculus) and rat (Rattus norvegicus) carcasses at our study site. Assuming that most pre-fledgling birds that die in the nest would be removed by parents or
siblings and become terrestrial carrion, our results indicate nearly a quarter would likely be scavenged by vertebrates. The amount of biomass ultimately consumed by vertebrate scavengers
would, however, likely depend on the size and/or age of the birds. For instance, our de facto young birds (i.e. quail carcasses) were removed by invertebrates at a significantly faster rate
than our “older carcasses” (i.e. chicken carcasses), yet we found both large and small carcasses were scavenged by vertebrates at relatively equal proportions. Given the high percentage of
nestling birds that die each year20, 24, even low levels of scavenging by vertebrates would represent an important pathway for energy transfer within these systems.
As a taxon, snakes were the dominant vertebrate scavengers, consuming 40% of scavenged carcasses, although this varied considerably by species. In general, snakes are uniquely suited to take
advantage of the patchy and ephemeral nature of carrion. Their relatively low maintenance metabolism allows them to survive on a few feedings per year35, and their unique physical
adaptations (e.g., no limbs, long slender body) would facilitate greater access to a variety of environments (e.g., burrows and tree canopies) less accessible to larger-bodied species. The
physiological adaptations of snakes, as well as other ectotherms, would tend to indicate a substantial fitness advantage from scavenging, while their physical adaptations would allow them to
occupy a niche that has traditionally been unassigned to other vertebrate taxon36, 37. Our results indicate at least some species of snakes (e.g., rat snakes) are ideally suited to access
both living and dead biomass from arboreal environments. We also suspect snakes accounted for the majority of unknown carcasses that disappeared from view between time lapse photos. Although
we placed carcasses on triggers, it was not uncommon for invertebrates to move them shortly after placement, and snake movement patterns were such that they rarely set off the motion sensor
of cameras. Future research should utilize cameras and monitoring techniques that better detect snakes and other ectothermic organisms in order to determine the fates of all carcasses and
minimize the amount of carcasses consumed by unknown scavengers.
Turkey vultures accounted for more scavenging events than any other species documented. Turkey vultures consumed a large proportion of arboreal carcasses in both years of our study,
scavenging 24% (n = 5) of the scavenged carcasses in 2015 and 21% (n = 3) in 2016. This is in stark contrast to rates reported for previous studies investigating fate of small mammal (rats
and mice) carrion, which have ranged from 0% to ~2% across several studies (e.g. refs 1, 7, 11 and 38). Although, within these same landscapes, Turner et al.11 and Olson et al.39 observed
extensive scavenging of larger carrion by vultures, leading the authors to conclude that mammals were more efficient scavengers of small carcasses than vultures11, 39. The surprisingly high
relative proportion of carcasses consumed by turkey vultures in our study is unlikely related to difference in mass compared to similar studies using small mammal carrion. For example, mouse
(19.4 g; SD = 1.7) and rat (228 g; SD = 52) carcasses used by DeVault et al.1 were more than twice the mass of our quail and chicken carcasses, respectively. Of the two carcass types we
deployed over the course of our study, we did observe that turkey vultures primarily consumed larger (i.e. chicken) carcasses (88%; Fig. 3), and three of the eight carcasses consumed (38%)
were taken from arboreal nests. Turkey vultures are known for their keen sense of smell40. Consequently, it is possible the placement of carcasses even a few feet above ground aided in
dispersing odors in such a way that they were more easily detected. Nevertheless, turkey vultures consumed similar proportions of carcasses from each habitat, removing 27% (n = 3) of
arboreal carcasses that were scavenged by vertebrates and 21% (n = 5) of scavenged terrestrial carcasses. Thus, location and possible improved odor dispersal alone would not account for the
10 to 20 fold increase in scavenging rates observed between our study and others with similarly sized carcasses conducted on the SRS. The underlying mechanism contributing to differences
among studies in turkey vulture scavenging of small carrion items is unknown, but could reflect increasing turkey vulture populations in the region41, possible differences in odor associated
with avian versus mammalian carcasses or other unknown factors that warrant further investigation.
In addition to nestling birds, adult birds and other tree-dwelling species (e.g., squirrels, raccoons) die from causes other than predation (e.g. disease) and become carrion resources. Some
of the carcasses will remain in trees (e.g. birds that die in cavities) while others may fall to the ground22. Scavenging is known to bring vertebrate species that do not typically interact
into contact as they move into the same areas to compete over carrion resources2. Considering the number of arboreal species present in forested ecosystems, carrion originating from trees
may be the basis of multiple links in food webs and connect arboreal, scansorial, and terrestrial vertebrates and could even connect arboreal species to burrowing species via invertebrates
that move carrion from the trees to, and beneath, the forest floor. As ecosystems are continually affected by natural and anthropogenic stressors, understanding how scavenging guilds
interact in heterogeneous environments to stabilize food webs, and ultimately influence ecosystem function, is increasingly important to understand. Our results highlight how even slight
variation in carcass placement allows a spectrum of species to access carrion differentially. We recommend future researchers obtaining information on exact animal locations following
mortality report these to allow for a more thorough assessment of the spatial variation of carrion availability, and inform additional empirical studies assessing its impact on scavenger
efficiency.
MethodsStudy AreaOur study was conducted at the Savannah River Site (SRS) near Aiken, South Carolina, USA. The SRS is a ~800 km2 site managed by the U.S. Department of Energy in the coastal plain region of
South Carolina and borders the Savannah River. The SRS was closed to the public for nuclear production in the 1950s42. Prior to 1951, settlers cleared forests to build homesteads, for timber
profit, and for conversion to agricultural land, specifically cotton and corn croplands42. A few areas remained unaltered, but after 1951 the USDA Forest Service restored much of the forest
and continues to manage the majority of the SRS for timber production of pine42. Currently, 54% of the land on the SRS is managed pine for timber production, 21% is riparian corridors and
wetland habitat, 11% is mixed forest, 9% is shrublands-grasslands, 3% is upland scrub forests, 2% is open water, and < 1% is developed for industrial purposes43. Industrial areas make up a
small fraction of the overall land area of the SRS and thus the site represents important habitat for a diversity of wildlife species42.
Field MethodsWe selected two habitat types (ground vs. tree) and two carcass sizes (quail chicks and chicken chicks) to represent potential bird carrion size/age class and habitat scenarios found in
nature. Frozen chicks of both species were ordered from an online store (RodentPro.com, LLC, Inglefield, IN) and a subset of carcasses were weighed; chicken carcasses ranged from 58.3–168.1
grams, with an average of 107.8 grams (SE = 3.5; n = 64) and quail carcasses weighed 4.8–11.1 grams, with an average of 8.1 grams (SE = 0.1; n = 82).
We conducted trials over two summers, 29 May to 29 July 2015 and 30 June to 27 July 2016, at nine mixed pine and hardwood stands separated by ≥1 km throughout the SRS. Our trials covered an
area of ~170 km2 in the western portion of SRS. We selected sites based on access and distributed them over multiple forested plots to try and encompass a range of vertebrate species and
densities. Trials were conducted using a randomized block design with the following possible trial types: 1) quail chick in a nest, 2) chicken chick on the ground, 3) quail chick on the
ground, and 4) chicken chick in a nest. Trial type was determined by randomly selecting a condition for the first trial of each set of four and moving in numerical order to the next trial
type, starting back at one after four.
Carcasses were monitored with Reconyx P9000 Hyperfire infrared, no-glow remote sensory cameras (RECONYX, Inc., Holmen, WI) equipped with a trigger system adapted from DeVault et al.1 that
forced the camera to take a picture upon removal of a carcass from the switch. Cameras were programmed to take a set of three pictures when triggered by motion or the external trigger and
wait 30 seconds before taking another set of images. Cameras were also programmed to take a time-lapse photo to monitor decomposition of carcasses between vertebrate scavenger visits, and in
case vertebrates did not find the carcass before it was fully decomposed by invertebrates and microbes. In 2015 we set time lapse to 15-minute intervals, and adjusted this to 5-minute
intervals in 2016 in an attempt to increase the probability of cameras capturing cold-blooded scavengers (i.e., snakes), as these species often would not trigger the motion sensor on the
cameras. For terrestrial carcasses, we placed carcasses on the ground near the base of trees (both hardwood and pine) and under the tree’s canopy in order to mimic locations where nestlings
may settle if removed from nests. We set trail cameras on a tree with the carcass in the center of the frame and camouflaged trigger cords with leaf litter. We placed arboreal carcasses in
artificial nests (Factory Direct Craft Supply Inc., Springboro, OH) 105–173.5 cm (average = 133.6; SE = 1.7; n = 59) off the ground in trees with dense low branching. Thus, carcasses placed
in tree nests were more indicative of low-canopy shrub-nesting species. We secured artificial nests with zip ties either on two branches or, if there were no branches sufficient to support
the nest, nails were hammered into the tree and used as artificial branches. Nests were surrounded by other branches with leaves. We attached trail cameras monitoring arboreal nests to an
adjacent tree positioned to look down onto the nest.
We placed a single carcass at each site and left carcasses in place for a minimum of six days before remains (if any) and the camera were recovered. Each active trial was at least 300 meters
away from any other active trials and 100 meters away from any previous trial sites that year to ensure vertebrate scavengers did not become behaviorally conditioned to visit any specific
site. We removed carcass remains (i.e. bones, feathers, flesh) at the end of the trial to prevent a lingering smell from attracting scavengers and biasing nearby trials. For trials that were
in trees, we removed nests and nails at the conclusion of each trial.
Camera AnalysisImages were downloaded from cameras after the conclusion of each trial and analyzed to determine: (1) the species, date, and time of all vertebrate scavenging events (i.e., vertebrate
feeding on but not removing the entire carcass), (2) date and time each carcass was removed by a vertebrate scavenger, and (3) length of time carcasses that were not scavenged by a
vertebrate persisted. A scavenging event was defined as any time a vertebrate consumed any part of the carcass. If we could not determine the exact species responsible for the scavenging
event we classified them as unknown. Trial images were analyzed from the time the carcass was placed until a maximum of six days. Images were used to assess carcass fate (scavenged by
vertebrates vs. decomposed by invertebrates), vertebrate scavenger species composition, and time to carcass removal. We did not attempt to quantify or categorize invertebrate species
responsible for carcass decomposition as there were often multiple species present, and any sort of quantification would have been subjective. Rather, we used our time-lapse photos to
determine that all carcasses not consumed by vertebrates were taken by invertebrates. All data generated or analyzed during this study are included in the Supplementary Information
files.
Statistical MethodsDifferences in carcass fate, vertebrate scavenger community composition, and time to carcass removal were analyzed across habitat types and carcasses sizes. We used program R44 to perform
all analyses. To assess differences in persistence time across habitat and carcass types, we used the survival package45 in program R to calculate probability of a carcass persisting through
time for each group (e.g., chicken carcasses on the ground, and in trees). We considered carcasses no longer available when they were removed from sight of the camera (e.g., carried under
leaf litter by invertebrates or out of camera view) or when it appeared invertebrates had removed all edible biomass. Time to removal was rounded to the nearest hour, and carcasses scavenged
by vertebrates were censored at time taken. We used a log-rank test to evaluate whether observed differences in removal times varied by group. The test computes a χ2 for observed and
expected events during each time step and tests the null hypothesis of no difference between curves.
We assessed time to removal for all carcasses taken by vertebrate scavengers using linear mixed effect models (LME) with the LME4 package46 in program R. Time to carcass removal was defined
as the elapsed time between carcass placement and complete carcass removal by a vertebrate scavenger. We analyzed removal times as a function of species (i.e., size) and habitat type from
all vertebrate-scavenged carcasses. Trials where no vertebrate scavenged the carcass were excluded from the analysis. We used time to removal (hrs) as the dependent variable, carcass and
habitat types as independent variables, and included year and site (i.e., one of the nine mixed hardwood or pine stands) as random effects. We constructed five models to assess differences
including a null model, both independent variables separately, and an additive and interactive effect of habitat and species. We used Akaike’s Information Criterion adjusted for small sample
size (AICc) to select models that best described the data. We compared AICc values to select the most parsimonious model and considered models differing by ≤2 AICc values as competitive47.
To evaluate differences in carcasses (1) scavenged vs (0) not scavenged by vertebrates, we used a generalized linear mixed effects model with the LME4 package47 in program R. We excluded all
trials where we could not definitively determine if the carcass was scavenged by a vertebrate. We compared the five identical models used for detection times, and evaluated them using the
same AICc framework.
To visualize the relationship between scavenger species and habitat and carcass types, we conducted a correspondence analysis (CA) using the R-package FactoMineR48. In our analysis, we used
scavenger species as rows and habitat (tree or ground) and carcass type (quail or chicken) as columns. We then populated the table based on the number of scavenging events recorded for each
scavenger species across all habitat type and carcass combinations. CA converts this frequency table data into a graphical display of the rows and columns and displays them as points, with
distances between points corresponding to Chi-squared distances49. In a typical 2 dimensional CA plot, each axis reflects a certain amount of the total variance and points contributing most
to the total variance fall furthest from the origin.
References DeVault, T. L., Brisbin, I. L. & Rhodes, O. E. Factors influencing the acquisition of rodent carrion by vertebrate scavengers and decomposers. Canadian Journal of Zoology-Revue
Canadienne De Zoologie 82, 502–509, doi:10.1139/z04-022 (2004).
Article Google Scholar
Barton, P. S., Cunningham, S. A., Lindenmayer, D. B. & Manning, A. D. The role of carrion in maintaining biodiversity and ecological processes in terrestrial ecosystems. Oecologia 171,
761–772 (2013).
Article ADS PubMed Google Scholar
Selva, N., Jędrzejewska, B., Jędrzejewski, W. & Wajrak, A. Factors affecting carcass use by a guild of scavengers in European temperate woodland. Canadian Journal of Zoology 83, 1590–1601
(2005).
Article Google Scholar
Wilson, E. E. & Wolkovich, E. M. Scavenging: how carnivores and carrion structure communities. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 26, 129–135, doi:10.1016/j.tree.2010.12.011 (2011).
Article Google Scholar
Mateo‐Tomás, P. et al. From regional to global patterns in vertebrate scavenger communities subsidized by big game hunting. Diversity and Distributions 21, 913–924 (2015).
Article Google Scholar
Payne, J. A. A summer carrion study of the baby pig Sus scrofa Linnaeus. Ecology 46, 592–602 (1965).
Article Google Scholar
DeVault, T. L., Olson, Z. H., Beasley, J. C. & Rhodes, O. E. Mesopredators dominate competition for carrion in an agricultural landscape. Basic and Applied Ecology 12, 268–274 (2011).
Article Google Scholar
DeVault, T. L., Rhodes, O. E. & Shivik, J. A. Scavenging by vertebrates: behavioral, ecological, and evolutionary perspectives on an important energy transfer pathway in terrestrial
ecosystems. Oikos 102, 225–234, doi:10.1034/j.1600-0706.2003.12378.x (2003).
Article Google Scholar
Parmenter, R. R. & MacMahon, J. A. Carrion decomposition and nutrient cycling in a semiarid shrub-steppe ecosystem. Ecological Monographs 79, 637–661, doi:10.1890/08-0972.1 (2009).
Article Google Scholar
Moleón, M., Sánchez‐Zapata, J. A., Sebastián‐González, E. & Owen‐Smith, N. Carcass size shapes the structure and functioning of an African scavenging assemblage. Oikos 124, 1391–1403 (2015).
Article Google Scholar
Turner, K. L., Abernethy, E. F., Conner, L. M., Rhodes, O. E. Jr., & Beasley, J. C. Abiotic and biotic factors modulate carrion fate and scavenging community dynamics. Ecology
doi:10.1002/ecy.1930 (2017).
Smith, J. A. et al. Fear of the human ‘super predator’reduces feeding time in large carnivores. Proc. R. Soc. B 284, 20170433 (2017).
Article PubMed Google Scholar
Selva, N. & Fortuna, M. A. The nested structure of a scavenger community. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences 274, 1101–1108 (2007).
Article Google Scholar
Beasley, J. C., Olson, Z. & DeVault, T. Carrion cycling in food webs: comparisons among terrestrial and marine ecosystems. Oikos 121, 1021–1026 (2012).
Article Google Scholar
Yang, L. H. Periodical cicadas as resource pulses in North American forests. Science 306, 1565–1567, doi:10.1126/science.1103114 (2004).
Article ADS CAS PubMed Google Scholar
Markandya, A. et al. Counting the cost of vulture decline—an appraisal of the human health and other benefits of vultures in India. Ecological economics 67, 194–204 (2008).
Article Google Scholar
Ogada, D., Torchin, M., Kinnaird, M. & Ezenwa, V. Effects of vulture declines on facultative scavengers and potential implications for mammalian disease transmission. Conservation Biology
26, 453–460 (2012).
Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar
Beasley, J. C., Olson, Z. H. & DeVault, T. L. Ecological role of vertebrate scavengers. Carrion ecology, evolution and their applications 107–127 (2015).
Forbes, S. L. & Carter, D. O. In Carrion Ecology, Evolution, and Their Applications 13–30 (CRC Press, 2015).
Young, H. Age-specific mortality in the eggs and nestlings of blackbirds. The Auk 80, 145–155 (1963).
Article Google Scholar
Elbroch, L. M. & Wittmer, H. U. Table scraps: inter-trophic food provisioning by pumas. Biology letters 8, 776–779 (2012).
Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
Smith, J. B. et al. Brown tree snake (Boiga irregularis) population density and carcass locations following exposure to acetaminophen. Ecotoxicology 25, 1556–1562 (2016).
Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar
Wellnitz, T. & Poff, N. L. Functional redundancy in heterogeneous environments: implications for conservation. Ecology Letters 4, 177–179 (2001).
Article Google Scholar
Arroyo, B. Sex‐biased nestling mortality in the Montagu’s harrier Circus pygargus. Journal of Avian Biology 33, 455–460 (2002).
Article Google Scholar
Hager, S. B., Cosentino, B. J. & McKay, K. J. Scavenging affects persistence of avian carcasses resulting from window collisions in an urban landscape. Journal of Field Ornithology 83,
203–211 (2012).
Article Google Scholar
Ponce, C., Alonso, J. C., Argandoña, G., García Fernández, A. & Carrasco, M. Carcass removal by scavengers and search accuracy affect bird mortality estimates at power lines. Animal
Conservation 13, 603–612 (2010).
Article Google Scholar
Villegas-Patraca, R., Macías-Sánchez, S., MacGregor-Fors, I. & Muñoz-Robles, C. Scavenger removal: bird and bat carcass persistence in a tropical wind farm. Acta oecologica 43, 121–125
(2012).
Article ADS Google Scholar
Mock, D. W., Schwagmeyer, P. & Dugas, M. B. Parental provisioning and nestling mortality in house sparrows. Animal Behaviour 78, 677–684 (2009).
Article Google Scholar
Viñuela, J. Sibling aggression, hatching asynchrony, and nestling mortality in the black kite (Milvus migrans). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 45, 33–45 (1999).
Article Google Scholar
Rippon, R., Alley, M. & Castro, I. Causes of mortality in a nestling population of free-living hihi (stitchbird—Notiomystis cincta). New Zealand Journal of Zoology 38, 207–222 (2011).
Article Google Scholar
Fleischli, M. A., Franson, J., Thomas, N., Finley, D. & Riley, W. Avian mortality events in the United States caused by anticholinesterase pesticides: a retrospective summary of National
Wildlife Health Center records from 1980 to 2000. Archives of environmental contamination and toxicology 46, 542–550 (2004).
Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar
Arnold, T. W. Uninformative Parameters and Model Selection Using Akaike’s Information Criterion. Journal of Wildlife Management 74, 1175–1178, doi:10.2193/2009-367 (2010).
Article Google Scholar
Fisher, R. J. & Wiebe, K. L. Nest site attributes and temporal patterns of northern flicker nest loss: effects of predation and competition. Oecologia 147, 744–753 (2006).
Article ADS PubMed Google Scholar
Li, P. & Martin, T. E. Nest-site selection and nesting success of cavity-nesting birds in high elevation forest drainages. The Auk, 405–418 (1991).
Beaupre, S. J. & Zaidan, F. III Scaling of CO2 production in the timber rattlesnake (Crotalus horridus), with comments on cost of growth in neonates and comparative patterns. Physiological
and Biochemical Zoology 74, 757–768 (2001).
Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar
Shivik, J. A. & Clark, L. Carrion seeking in brown tree snakes: importance of olfactory and visual cues. (1997).
DeVault, T. L. & Krochmal, A. R. Scavenging by snakes: an examination of the literature. Herpetologica 58, 429–436 (2002).
Article Google Scholar
Olson, Z., Beasley, J., DeVault, T. L. & Rhodes, O. Scavenger community response to the removal of a dominant scavenger. Oikos 121, 77–84 (2012).
Article Google Scholar
Olson, Z. H., Beasley, J. C. & Rhodes, O. E. Jr. Carcass type affects local scavenger guilds more than habitat connectivity. PloS one 11, e0147798 (2016).
Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
Buckley, N. J. Food finding and the influence of information, local enhancement, and communal roosting on foraging success of North American vultures. The Auk, 473–488 (1996).
Avery, M. L. Trends in North American vulture populations. USDA National Wildlife Research Center-Staff Publications, 75 (2004).
White, D. L. & Gaines, K. F. The Savannah River Site: site description, land use, and management history (2000).
Therneau, T. A package for survival analysis in S. R package version 2.37-4. See http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=survival (2014).
Beasley, J. C., Grazia, T. E., Johns, P. E. & Mayer, J. J. Habitats associated with vehicle collisions with wild pigs. Wildlife Research 40, 654–660 (2014).
Article Google Scholar
Team, R. R development core team. RA Lang Environ Stat Comput 55, 275–286 (2013).
Google Scholar
Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B. & Walker, S. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. arXiv preprint arXiv:1406.5823 (2014).
Burnham, K. P. & Anderson, D. Model selection and multi-model inference. A Pratical informatio-theoric approch. Sringer 1229 (2003).
Lê, S., Josse, J. & Husson, F. FactoMineR: an R package for multivariate analysis. Journal of statistical software 25, 1–18 (2008).
Article Google Scholar
Greenacre, M. J. Theory and applications of correspondence analysis (1984).
Download references
AcknowledgementsWe thank D. Keiter for field assistance, and S. Webster, C. Leaphart, E. Borchert, P. Schlichting for comments on earlier drafts of the manuscript. This work was supported through
Cooperative Agreements between the University of Georgia Research Foundation and the United States Department of Energy (No. DE-EM0004391). Additional funding was provided by the National
Science Foundation through award DBI Award 1460940 to the University of Georgia’s Research Foundation.
Author informationAuthors and Affiliations University of Georgia, Savannah River Ecology Lab, PO Box Drawer E, Aiken, SC, 29802, USA
Joshua B. Smith
University of Georgia, Savannah River Ecology Lab, Warnell School of Forestry and Natural Resources, PO Box Drawer E, Aiken, SC, 29802, USA
Lauren J. Laatsch & James C. Beasley
AuthorsJoshua B. SmithView author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar
Lauren J. LaatschView author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar
James C. BeasleyView author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar
ContributionsJ.B. Smith and J.C. Beasley conceived and designed the experiment. J.B. Smith and L.J. Laatsch collected field data. J.B. Smith, J.C. Bealsey, and L.J. Laatsch analyzed the data. J.B. Smith,
J.C. Bealsey, and L.J. Laatsch wrote the paper.
Corresponding author Correspondence to Joshua B. Smith.
Ethics declarations Competing InterestsThe authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Additional informationPublisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Electronic supplementary materialSupplementaryTable S1 and Supplementary Table S2Dataset 1Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or
format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or
other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in
the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the
copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
Reprints and permissions
About this articleCite this article Smith, J.B., Laatsch, L.J. & Beasley, J.C. Spatial complexity of carcass location influences vertebrate scavenger efficiency and species composition. Sci
Rep 7, 10250 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-10046-1
Download citation
Received: 03 April 2017
Accepted: 02 August 2017
Published: 31 August 2017
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-10046-1
Share this article Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:
Get shareable link Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.
Copy to clipboard Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative