Gender inequality in cum laude distinctions for phd students

Gender inequality in cum laude distinctions for phd students

Play all audios:

Loading...

ABSTRACT Resource allocation in academia is highly skewed, and peer evaluation is the main method used to distribute scarce resources. A large literature documents gender inequality in


evaluation, and the explanation for this inequality is homophily: male evaluators give more favorable ratings to male candidates. We investigate this by focusing on cum laude distinctions


for PhD students in the Netherlands, a distinction that is only awarded to 5 percent of all dissertations and has as its sole goal to distinguish the top from the rest. Using data from over


5000 PhD recipients of a large Dutch university for the period 2011–2021, we find that female PhD students were almost two times less likely to get a cum laude distinction than their male


counterparts, even when they had the same doctoral advisor. This gender gap is largest when dissertations are evaluated by all-male committees and decreases as evaluation committees include


more female members. SIMILAR CONTENT BEING VIEWED BY OTHERS BIAS IN PHYSICS PEER RECOGNITION DOES NOT EXPLAIN GAPS IN PERCEIVED PEER RECOGNITION Article 05 March 2025 GENDER DIVERSITY OF


RESEARCH CONSORTIA CONTRIBUTES TO FUNDING DECISIONS IN A MULTI-STAGE GRANT PEER-REVIEW PROCESS Article Open access 10 June 2022 GENDER DISPARITIES IN SOCIAL AND PERSONALITY PSYCHOLOGY AWARDS


FROM 1968 TO 2021 Article Open access 03 July 2024 INTRODUCTION Resource allocation in science is highly skewed1,2,3. Acceptance rates at high-prestige journals are often below ten percent,


and the same holds true for prestigious research grants4. Consequently, only a small fraction of all researchers can publish in high-prestige journals, receive research grants, or obtain


tenured positions. Peer evaluation is key to determining who receives these resources: scholars review each other’s work and determine who belongs to the top and who does not5. While peer


evaluation is the dominant method to divide scarce resources, it is not uncontested. A large literature has investigated gender inequality in peer evaluation across a multitude of settings.


Many studies find evidence for inequality: journal peer review scores are lower for women6,7,8, men receive higher scores for their grant proposals9,10,11, teaching evaluations are less


favorable for women12,13,14,15, and men are more likely to be evaluated positively in academic hiring16,17. The literature is not uniform in this finding, with some recent studies, for


example, finding no gender inequality in journal peer review18,19 or academic hiring20. However, gender inequality in evaluation is argued to be part of the explanation for gender


differences in academic careers21. Why do female scholars receive lower evaluations than their male counterparts? Quality could be a first argument, but several studies show that differences


remain even when quality is kept constant11,15,22. Another important argument lies in the subjective nature of evaluations: evaluations are never objective and cannot be dissociated from


evaluators5,23. Evaluators’ conception of academic excellence is not neutral but gendered24,25, and they are more likely to perceive men as top achievers. Recent studies also find evidence


for gender bias in group work, where women receive less credit for their contributions22. Homophily between evaluator and evaluated is also argued to be one of the key reasons for gender


inequality in evaluations8,18,26,27. Particularly in contexts where there are no neutral or universal criteria that distinguish the top from the rest, evaluators are more favorable about


those that are similar to themselves23. Since the likelihood to evaluate colleagues (e.g. in hiring committees or grant panels) increases with seniority, the strong overrepresentation of men


amongst senior scholars24,28 could reproduce existing gender inequalities when male evaluators are more likely to evaluate male academics more positively. In this article, we study gender


inequality in evaluation at the very start of the academic career, by looking at the association between gender and cum laude distinctions for PhD students in the Netherlands. After


defending their dissertation, PhD students either obtain a regular doctoral degree or a doctoral degree with a cum laude distinction. Obtaining this distinction is rare: only about 5% of all


PhD students are awarded a cum laude. The main doctoral advisor (always a full professor) initiates the procedure to award a cum laude (see Supplementary Appendix for a detailed description


of the procedure). A separate dissertation committee consisting of five to seven scholars with relevant expertise then decides. The committee must unanimously agree that the dissertation


should be awarded cum laude. If the committee votes for cum laude, two external referees (that are provided by the doctoral advisor) must attest this to be the case. If they all believe the


dissertation to rank among the top 5%, a cum laude is awarded. PhD students have no influence on this process and are not informed. Only after defending their dissertation, they will learn


whether their dissertation was awarded a cum laude distinction. Candidates who do not receive the award will generally never learn whether a cum laude procedure was initiated. Some earlier


studies investigated gender inequality in doctoral education. Research in Norway showed that men are more likely to enroll in doctorate education25. Similar results were found for Germany29,


but this study showed that academic performance only to a very limited extent explained the gender difference in enrollment. This means that while men were more likely to enroll in doctoral


programs, there was no large quality difference between them. A recent study, also in Germany, did show that female doctoral students were slightly less likely to get the summa cum laude


recognition for their dissertation30. The empirical case of cum laude distinctions for PhD students provides an interesting context to study gender inequality in evaluations for three


reasons. First, there are no clear criteria that define a cum laude distinction—this all depends on what evaluators perceive to be important. This absence of any formal criteria (see


Supplementary Appendix for more information) thereby provides a most likely case to observe gender inequality in evaluations. It also allows us to study the role of homophily: most doctoral


advisors and members of doctoral committees are male, and we investigate whether male evaluators are more likely to evaluate male PhD students as excellent. Second, since most doctoral


advisors have supervised multiple PhD students over the period of observation (2010–2021), we can exploit fixed effects models31 where we study gender differences in the likelihood of


obtaining cum laude or homophily effects of the dissertation committee within the same doctoral advisor. This allows us to cancel out explanations that point to between-advisor variation,


for example in supervision quality, or their likelihood to apply for cum laude. Finally, the only purpose of a cum laude is to separate the exceptional from the rest. This makes the awarding


of cum laude distinctions one of the clearest manifestations of the search for excellence that permeates academia32,33. For our analyses, we rely on the information from 5239 PhD students


who received their doctorate between 2011 and 2021 at a Dutch public university that is one of the largest research universities in Europe. All PhD students who have defended their


dissertations in the mentioned period are part of the sample and were analyzed with data from a university-wide population database. The university under study covers all scientific domains


and disciplines. More information on the data collection and processing can be found in the Supplementary Appendix. RESULTS The data reveal large gender inequality in the probability to


obtain a doctoral degree with a cum laude distinction. Figure 1 shows that 6.57% of all male PhD students obtained cum laude, compared to 3.68% of all female students. Compared to


dissertations of female PhD students, dissertations of male PhD students are 1.8 times (_χ_2 test, _p_ < 0.001, N = 5239) more likely to be considered as belonging to the top 5%. This


large gender inequality in cum laude distinctions does not necessarily means that male and female PhD students are evaluated differently. Evaluation practices and definitions of quality vary


strongly across scientific domains23,32. The observed inequality in Fig. 1 therefore could also be explained by a Simpson’s paradox34: female scholars are more likely to obtain their PhD in


academic fields where cum laude distinctions are rarely given. Earlier research has for example shown that gender inequality in grant funding35 or citations36 can be explained by such a


paradox. Figure 2 evaluates whether differences across academic fields can explain the observed gender gap in cum laude distinctions. The left panel shows the association between the


proportion of female PhD students and the likelihood to get a cum laude distinction across different academic fields. A systematic pattern is absent. In Medicine, the probability to receive


a cum laude distinction is comparatively lowest (3.6%), while it is highest in the Social Sciences (9.2%). In both fields most PhD students are female. If anything, it seems that cum laude


is more often awarded in academic fields with more female PhD students. If the gender inequality in Fig. 1 was explained by a Simpson’s paradox, we would expect the exact opposite. Figure 2


(right panel) shows that the gender gap in cum laude is apparent across all six academic domains. In line with earlier studies18, one might hypothesize that the female disadvantage in cum


laude would be largest in the academic fields that include the least female academics (i.e. science, economics). While the magnitude of the gap differs, these cross-field differences are not


statistically significant (_p_ > 0.05). The absence of a Simpson’s paradox is confirmed by regression analyses (Fig. 3, left-most markers) where the male advantage in cum laude


distinctions remains stable after controlling for academic field (_p_ < 0.001). Differences across academic fields do not explain the large gender disparity observed in Fig. 1. It might


be that the definition of fields used in Fig. 2 is too broad, for example, if there are selection effects into more narrow academic fields36 or even doctoral advisors. If the supervisory


quality of doctoral advisors or their likelihood to apply for a cum laude correlates with the likelihood to supervise a female PhD student, this might explain the observed gender inequality.


Figure 3 (right-most markers) presents the results of a regression analysis that includes a fixed effect31 for doctoral advisor (see Supplementary Appendix). This estimate can be


interpreted as the average within-advisor gender gap in cum laude distinctions. It indicates whether male and female PhD students that were supervised by the same doctoral advisor had a


different probability for a cum laude distinction. In this fixed effects specification, the large gender gap is unchanged (_p_ < 0.001). Dissertations of female PhD students are, on


average, almost twice less likely to get the mark of excellence than their male counterparts with the same doctoral advisor. To what extent do we observe gender-driven homophily in cum laude


distinctions? Fig. 4 evaluates this by looking both at the gender of the doctoral supervisors (left panel) and the gender composition of the committee that evaluates the dissertation (right


panel). In both figures the y-axis presents the female-male gap in cum laude distinctions in percentage points (_pp_). For doctoral advisors (Fig. 3, left panel), we find little evidence


for gender bias homophily. Female PhD students are less likely than male PhD students to obtain cum laude irrespective of whether their doctoral advisor is male (3.6_ pp_ less likely) or


female (2.3_ pp_ less likely). While the gender gap in cum laude distinctions is smaller when the advisor is female, this difference between male and female doctoral advisors is not


statistically significant (_p_ = 0.38). The dissertation committee plays a crucial role in awarding the cum laude distinction too: they must agree that the dissertation belongs to the top


5%. For the committee, we find a pattern of gender homophily (Fig. 4, right panel). These results are again based on a fixed effects specification and exploit the variation in committee


composition _within_ the same doctoral advisor. There is no exogenous variation in committees37, and doctoral advisors have an important say in determining the composition of the committee.


However, the fixed effects approach rules out that the observed pattern of gender homophily stems from variation across doctoral advisors: all effects are estimated within doctoral advisors.


The gender gap in cum laude is largest when the dissertation is evaluated by an all-male committee: 5.8_ pp_. In this scenario, 9.1% of male PhD students obtain a cum laude distinction,


compared to 3.3% of female PhD students with the same doctoral advisor—a difference by a factor of almost 3 (see Supplementary Appendix Fig. S5). The gender gap in cum laude distinctions


decreases as the gender composition shifts towards a greater number of female committee members. For dissertations that are evaluated by a committee where at least a quarter of the members


is female, the gender gap in cum laude distinctions disappears. More balanced dissertation committees were a minority. Over the observed period, 9.6% of all dissertations were evaluated by a


committee where more than half of the members were female, whereas 27.8% were evaluated by an all-male committee (see Supplementary Appendix Table S1). We have performed several robustness


checks. First, some studies have argued that gender inequality in evaluation is decreasing over time38, additional analyses show that the found pattern of gender inequality in cum laude


distinctions remains stable over the observed period (2011–2021) (see Supplementary Appendix Fig. S8). Second, the seniority of committee members might explain the homophily results here,


given that female committee members might be less likely to be senior. We do not find evidence for this, and the presented results are robust when we include committee members’ seniority


(see Supplementary Appendix Table S6). Finally, next to doctoral advisors, PhD students often have co-supervisors (see Supplementary Appendix). They are involved in supervision but do not


have the formal right to hand out doctoral degrees. Our finding that there are no homophily effects for doctoral advisors holds when we look at the gender composition of the full supervisory


team (see Supplementary Appendix Fig. S9). DISCUSSION We conclude that there is substantial inequality in the extent to which the work of early researchers is evaluated and perceived as


excellent. Male PhD students are almost twice as likely than female PhD students to obtain a cum laude distinction. The gender of the doctoral advisors is not significantly associated with


the size of the gap, but the gender composition of the committee is: the average gender gap in cum laude distinctions is largest for all-male committees and nears zero when committees are


getting closer to gender parity. A limitation of the current analysis is that we do not know which committee member has voted for or against cum laude, which means that we were only able to


estimate homophily effects in the composition of the evaluation team. Moreover, we do not have any information on the two external anonymous reviewers that also have to attest that the


dissertation deserves cum laude. Although the names of these external referees are provided by the doctoral advisor(s), we cannot rule out that they also contribute to the observed gender


inequality in cum laude distinctions. Earlier studies also pointed to the unequal effects of parenthood to understand gender inequality in academia39. While we do not have data on


parenthood, only a small fraction of PhD students become parents during the writing of their dissertation40,41. In this study, we were also not able to investigate whether other aspects of


the work relationship between PhD student and doctoral advisor (e.g. if they taught together) plays a role. A final limitation of the current study is that it is based on one university.


While it might be that gender inequality differs across universities in the Netherlands, the studied university is broad and not limited to a few academic fields. Moreover, recently, a


newspaper reported gender inequality in cum laude across most Dutch universities42. A puzzling finding is that there are homophily effects for the committee but not for the doctoral


advisors. The data do not allow us to explore this finding further. A possible explanation might be that supervisors build stronger relationships with their students than committee members,


which makes their assessment more accurate. Most prominently, with the current data we are unable to fully rule out that the observed gender inequality in cum laude distinctions is explained


by something else than gender bias43. It might for example be that male PhD students are overrepresented in the right tail of the “quality distribution”. Existing research provides little


evidence for this. Among school-aged children, both boys and girls are overrepresented in the right tail depending on which skill is tested (numeracy or literacy)44,45. To isolate the part


of the effect that is driven by bias would be to “control” for the quality of the PhD dissertation (e.g. publications, citations for dissertational work). However, this will not help us


further since these measures of quality are themselves affected by gender bias21,43,46. Another alternative explanation for our findings might be found in a gender gap in


competitiveness47,48. PhD students are not aware that their doctoral advisors apply for cum laude and have no influence over this decision. It is therefore highly unlikely that the gender


gap in cum laude distinctions is driven by differences in strategic choices concerning the application process49. Moreover, the reported gender gaps in competitiveness are much smaller than


the effects found. While we are unable to interpret the observed gender inequality as bias, we do believe that our results provide strong indications for a biased perception of excellence.


The gender gap in cum laude is not explained by differences across academic fields or doctoral advisors: within the same doctoral advisors, male PhD students are almost two times more likely


to obtain a cum laude. Nevertheless, only in a controlled experiment where quality is kept constant, we would be able to identify the causal effect of gender on cum laude distinctions. This


is not possible in the current study, and its main contribution was to document the association between gender and the cum laude distinction. Future studies should try to triangulate the


finding in more controlled designs. How big is the career advantage for those who obtain a cum laude distinction? In other words, how important is it? While it is difficult to quantify this,


graduating cum laude is an official criterion of the largest Dutch grant funder to signal the quality of the researcher. More generally, it is used as a mark of excellence, particularly in


job applications for early career researchers. We believe that the findings of this study bear relevance besides the importance of cum laude, as the current study shows that already in the


early career there is a large gender inequality in who is perceived as excellent. Men are more often deemed to belong to the top than women, particularly when evaluated by men. Recent


studies indicate that gender inequality in academia is pervasive21,50. This study is no exception. Cum laude distinctions might be functional for those who obtain them, as they obtain the


benefits associated with them. At the same time, the cum laude distinction is an instrument that leads to inequalities that are unlikely meritocratic in nature. In line with earlier work49,


this research indicates that gender inequality in academia is at least to some extent driven by institutional barriers that tend to be higher for women than for men. Obtaining a cum laude


distinction is such a barrier, that is easier to pass for men than for women. How to solve this inequality? Double-blind reviewing is seen as one way to mitigate gender inequality51, but in


the context of evaluating dissertations, this will be difficult as by definition the majority of committee members work at the same university and is likely to know the PhD student. The


current study presents evidence for gender bias homophily in evaluators. A straightforward solution would therefore be to enhance the gender balance in evaluation committees in academia.


However, given the gender segregation across academic fields (see Fig. 2, left panel), for some academic disciplines this will be unachievable in the short term—or it would put even greater


pressure on the small group of female academics in those fields. We believe that this study raises a more fundamental question: is it always crucial to distinguish the excellent from the


rest? In some cases, such as hiring, it is unavoidable: there are not enough jobs for everyone. But academia is rife with prizes, awards, and distinctions, that often serve no other purpose


than marking some as excellent. We can question whether these institutional barriers provide large functional benefits for academia, but the current study does show that they can perpetuate


existing inequalities. Debates about bias in the evaluation of excellence therefore should not just be about how to create equal opportunities when men and women face institutional barriers


in academia, but also about the necessity of institutional barriers in the first place. MATERIALS AND METHODS DATA In this study, we analyze information from one of the largest public


universities in The Netherlands. The data contained information on all PhD students that completed their dissertations at the university between 2011 and 2021. In total, 5240 dissertations


were written over this period. All dissertations are part of the data, which means that we analyze population data for the university. The university under study is one of the largest


research universities in Europe. It employs over 3000 researchers and is home to 40,000 students. The university is organized into seven faculties that cover the breadth of academic


research: Medicine, Law, Social Sciences (e.g. Sociology, Psychology), Humanities (e.g. Philosophy, Language, History), Economics and Business, and Science (e.g. Physics, Mathematics,


Biology). All large academic disciplines are represented in the university, with engineering as the exception. In the Netherlands, Engineering Schools can be found in the three technical


universities, and hence PhD dissertations written in the technical domain were not part of the data. The data consists of 5239 dissertations—one observation is set to missing because no


doctoral advisors were registered. PhD students can have multiple doctoral advisors, and many doctoral advisors have supervised multiple PhD students in the observed period (2011–2021) (see


Fig. S4 in the Supplementary Appendix). In our data, we identified 1623 unique doctoral advisors _j_, that have supervised a total of 5239 PhD students _i_. In total, there were 7249


doctoral advisor–PhD student combinations _ij_ in our data. In the regression analyses, we analyzed different samples, depending on the method or research question. For example, we analyzed


the sample of PhD students (N=5239) when we were interested in variation between PhD students (for example in calculating the raw difference in cum laude distinctions between men and women),


but the sample of all PhD student–doctoral advisor combinations (N=7249) were analyzed when we were interested in the role of the doctoral advisors or committee. We have collected different


information on the dissertation and the PhD student from the university databases. First, for each dissertation we knew the gender of the PhD student, the date of obtaining the PhD, the


broad academic field the dissertation was written in, and whether the dissertation was awarded cum laude. Second, for the supervisors, we knew their roles in the supervision (doctoral


advisor or co-supervisor), names, gender, and academic title. This latter information allows us to separate full professors from assistant and associate professors, as only the first is


allowed to carry the “Prof.” title. The academic title thereby is a good proxy of the seniority of the doctoral advisor (or co-supervisor). Finally, the data contained information on the


dissertation committee. For them, we have collected their gender and academic title. Table S1 in the Supplementary Appendix provides an overview of all measures used in the study.


STATISTICAL ANALYSES Some results in the article are descriptive, but others are obtained by performing different types of regression analyses. For all analyses, we estimated a linear


probability model52,53 on our binary dependent variable _y__i_, which measures whether the dissertation of PhD student _i_ did (1) or did not (0) receive a cum laude distinction. While we


use linear probability models, logistic regressions provide the same findings (see Supplementary Appendix Fig. S10). We prefer linear probability models over logistic regression because


several studies have highlighted difficulties with calculating standard errors and interpreting interaction effects in logit (and probit) models54,55. To obtain the results presented in the


article, we use three different regression models, depending on whether we analyze the sample of PhD students _i_ or the sample of all combinations between PhD students _i_ and doctoral


advisors _j_. First, we have used a simple linear regression at the level of PhD students (N=5239) to estimate the results in Fig. 1 and the left estimates in Fig. 3. Here we regressed


obtaining cum laude on the gender of the PhD student. In this model, we could add several controls measured at the level of the PhD student (year of obtained PhD; academic field). Details on


the model and the estimated results can be found in the Supplementary Appendix. Second, we have estimated multilevel regressions on the sample of all combinations between PhD students (_i_)


and doctoral advisors (_j_) (N=7239). We used these models because we wanted to know whether gender inequality in cum laude distinctions differed across male and female doctoral advisors


(Fig. 4, left panel). The data structure is cross classified: doctoral advisors had multiple PhD students, but PhD students also had multiple doctoral advisors. To correct for this we


cluster standard errors within PhD students. Details on the equation and the estimated results can be found in the Supplementary Appendix. Finally, we used the combined sample of doctoral


advisors (N=7239) to estimate fixed effects models. By including a fixed effect for the doctoral advisor _j_, all between-doctoral advisor variance is accounted for, and the only variance


that is left to explain was that within each advisor. This means that we estimated whether, on average, the same doctoral advisor was more likely to have male than female PhD students with


cum laude distinctions (Fig. 3, right estimate). In a similar way, we exploited the variation in the gender composition of the dissertation committees within the same doctoral advisors and


estimated whether, on average, for the same supervisors the gender inequality in cum laude depended on the percentage of female members of the dissertation committee (Fig. 4, right panel).


An important benefit of these models is that we were able to rule out the variation between doctoral advisors as a source of variation for gender inequality in cum laude. More information on


the fixed effects models and their assumptions, as well as the obtained results, can be found in the Supplementary Appendix. The current study and data collection has been approved by the


Ethical Committee of the Amsterdam Institute for Social Science Research of the University of Amsterdam. All methods were carried out in accordance with the guidelines and regulations of the


University of Amsterdam. The need for informed consent was waived by the Ethical Committee of the Amsterdam Institute for Social Science Research of the University of Amsterdam. DATA


AVAILABILITY All code and data are available via https://osf.io/3fqe2/. The data are only available in anonymized form. Full data access (including identifiable information) will only be


possible when granted by the supplier of the university that was studied. REFERENCES * Nielsen, M. W. & Andersen, J. P. Global citation inequality is on the rise. _Proc. Natl. Acad.


Sci._ 118, e2012208118 (2021). Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Reskin, B. F. Scientific productivity and the reward structure of science. _Am. Social. Rev._ 42,


491–504 (1977). Article  Google Scholar  * Petersen, A. M., Jung, W.-S., Yang, J.-S. & Stanley, H. E. Quantitative and empirical demonstration of the Matthew effect in a study of career


longevity. _Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci._ 108, 18–23 (2011). Article  ADS  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Bol, T., De Vaan, M. & Van de Rijt, A. The Matthew effect in science funding. _PNAS_


115, 4887–4890 (2018). Article  ADS  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Lee, C. J., Sugimoto, C. R., Zhang, G. & Cronin, B. Bias in peer review. _J. Am. Soc. Inf. Sci.


Technol._ 64, 2–17 (2013). Article  Google Scholar  * Wenneras, C. & Wold, A. Nepotism and sexism in peer-review. _Nature_ 387, 6631 (1997). Article  Google Scholar  *


Knobloch-Westerwick, S., Glynn, C. J. & Huge, M. The Matilda effect in science communication: An experiment on gender bias in publication quality perceptions and collaboration interest.


_Sci. Commun._ 35, 603–625 (2013). Article  Google Scholar  * Murray, D. _et al._ Author-reviewer homophily in peer review. _bioRxiv_ https://doi.org/10.1101/400515 (2019). Article  Google


Scholar  * Bornmann, L., Mutz, R. & Daniel, H.-D. Gender differences in grant peer review: A meta-analysis. _J. Informet._ 1, 226–238 (2007). Article  Google Scholar  * Bol, T., de Vaan,


M. & van de Rijt, A. Gender-equal funding rates conceal unequal evaluations. _Res. Policy_ 51, 104399 (2022). Article  Google Scholar  * Witteman, H. O., Hendricks, M., Straus, S. &


Tannenbaum, C. Are gender gaps due to evaluations of the applicant or the science? A natural experiment at a national funding agency. _Lancet_ 393, 531–540 (2019). Article  PubMed  Google


Scholar  * Mengel, F., Sauermann, J. & Zölitz, U. Gender bias in teaching evaluations. _J. Eur. Econ. Assoc._ 17, 535–566 (2019). Article  Google Scholar  * Storage, D., Horne, Z.,


Cimpian, A. & Leslie, S.-J. The frequency of “brilliant” and “genius” in teaching evaluations predicts the representation of women and African Americans across fields. _PloS One_ 11,


e0150194 (2016). Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Boring, A. Gender biases in student evaluations of teaching. _J. Public Econ._ 145, 27–41 (2017). Article  Google Scholar


  * Rivera, L. A. & Tilcsik, A. Scaling down inequality: Rating scales, gender bias, and the architecture of evaluation. _Am. Sociol. Rev._ 84, 248–274 (2019). Article  Google Scholar  *


Dutt, K., Pfaff, D. L., Bernstein, A. F., Dillard, J. S. & Block, C. J. Gender differences in recommendation letters for postdoctoral fellowships in geoscience. _Nat. Geosci._ 9, 805


(2016). Article  ADS  CAS  Google Scholar  * Moss-Racusin, C. A., Dovidio, J. F., Brescoll, V. L., Graham, M. J. & Handelsman, J. Science faculty’s subtle gender biases favor male


students. _Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci._ 109, 16474–16479 (2012). Article  ADS  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Squazzoni, F. _et al._ Peer review and gender bias: A study on 145


scholarly journals. _Sci. Adv._ 7, eabd0299 (2021). Article  ADS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Fox, C. W. & Paine, C. T. Gender differences in peer review outcomes and


manuscript impact at six journals of ecology and evolution. _Ecol. Evol._ 9, 3599–3619 (2019). Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Lutter, M. & Schröder, M. Who becomes a


tenured professor, and why? Panel data evidence from German sociology, 1980–2013. _Res. Policy_ 45, 999–1013 (2016). Article  Google Scholar  * Larivière, V., Ni, C., Gingras, Y., Cronin, B.


& Sugimoto, C. R. Bibliometrics: Global gender disparities in science. _Nature_ 504, 211–213 (2013). Article  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Sarsons, H., Gërxhani, K., Reuben, E. &


Schram, A. Gender differences in recognition for group work. _J. Polit. Econ._ 129, 101–147 (2021). Article  Google Scholar  * Lamont, M. _How Professors Think_ (Harvard University Press,


2009). Book  Google Scholar  * Commission, E. _She Figures: Gender in Research and Innovation_ (European Commission, 2013). Google Scholar  * Mastekaasa, A. Gender differences in educational


attainment: The case of doctoral degrees in Norway. _Br. J. Sociol. Educ._ 26, 375–394 (2005). Article  Google Scholar  * Sheltzer, J. M. & Smith, J. C. Elite male faculty in the life


sciences employ fewer women. _Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci._ 111, 10107–10112 (2014). Article  ADS  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Teplitskiy, M., Acuna, D., Elamrani-Raoult, A.,


Körding, K. & Evans, J. The sociology of scientific validity: How professional networks shape judgement in peer review. _Res. Policy_ 47, 1825–1841 (2018). Article  Google Scholar  *


Clauset, A., Arbesman, S. & Larremore, D. B. Systematic inequality and hierarchy in faculty hiring networks. _Sci. Adv._ 1, e1400005 (2015). Article  ADS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google


Scholar  * Lörz, M. & Mühleck, K. Gender differences in higher education from a life course perspective: Transitions and social inequality between enrolment and first post-doc position.


_High. Educ._ 77, 381–402 (2019). Article  Google Scholar  * de Vogel, S. Simply the best? Determinants of achieving the highest grade in a doctoral degree in Germany. _High. Educ._ 85,


1161–1180 (2023). Article  Google Scholar  * Allison, P. D. _Fixed Effects Regression Models_ Vol. 160 (SAGE publications, 2009). Book  Google Scholar  * van den Brink, M. & Benschop, Y.


Gender practices in the construction of academic excellence: Sheep with five legs. _Organization_ 19, 507–524 (2012). Article  Google Scholar  * O’Connor, P. & O’Hagan, C. Excellence in


university academic staff evaluation: A problematic reality?. _Stud. High. Educ._ 41, 1943–1957 (2016). Article  Google Scholar  * Bickel, P. J., Hammel, E. A. & O’Connell, J. W. Sex


bias in graduate admissions: Data from Berkeley: Measuring bias is harder than is usually assumed, and the evidence is sometimes contrary to expectation. _Science_ 187, 398–404 (1975).


Article  ADS  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Albers, C. J. Dutch research funding, gender bias, and Simpson’s paradox. _PNAS_ 112, E6828–E6829 (2015). Article  ADS  CAS  PubMed  PubMed


Central  Google Scholar  * Tekles, A., Auspurg, K. & Bornmann, L. Same-gender citations do not indicate a substantial gender homophily bias. _PLoS One_ 17, e0274810 (2022). Article  CAS


  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * De Paola, M. & Scoppa, V. Gender discrimination and evaluators’ gender: Evidence from Italian academia. _Economica_ 82, 162–188 (2015).


Article  Google Scholar  * Ceci, S. J. & Williams, W. M. Understanding current causes of women’s underrepresentation in science. _Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci._ 108, 3157–3162 (2011). Article 


ADS  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Morgan, A. C. _et al._ The unequal impact of parenthood in academia. _Sci. Adv._ 7, eabd1996 (2021). Article  ADS  PubMed  PubMed Central


  Google Scholar  * Bol, T., Derks, B. & Poorthuis, L. _The Impact of the COVID-19 Pandemic First Lockdown Period on the Work and Well-being of Academics in The Netherlands_ (De Jonge


Akademie/LNVH Amsterdam/Utrecht, 2021). Google Scholar  * Central Bureau of Statistics, “Resultaten Gepromoveerdenonderzoek 2019” (Report, The Hague, the Netherlands, 2020).


https://www.cbs.nl/nl-nl/achtergrond/2020/14/resultaten-gepromoveerdenonderzoek-2019). * E. de Bruin, Helft van de promovendi is vrouw, maar cum laude krijgen ze zelden. _NRC Handelsblad,


October_, 20 (2018). * Traag, V.A., Waltman, L. Causal foundations of bias, disparity and fairness (2022). doi:https://doi.org/10.48550/arXiv.2207.13665. * Machin, S. & Pekkarinen, T.


Global sex differences in test score variability. _Science_ 322, 1331–1332 (2008). Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Reilly, D., Neumann, D. L. & Andrews, G. Gender differences in


reading and writing achievement: Evidence from the National Assessment of Educational Progress (NAEP). _Am. Psychol._ 74, 445 (2019). Article  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Way, S.F., Larremore,


D.B., Clauset, A. Gender, productivity, and prestige in computer science faculty hiring networks. In _Proc. of the 25th International Conference on World Wide Web_, 1169–1179 (2016). *


Buser, T., Niederle, M. & Oosterbeek, H. Gender, competitiveness, and career choices. _Q. J. Econ._ 129, 1409–1447 (2014). Article  Google Scholar  * Bosquet, C., Combes, P.-P. &


García-Peñalosa, C. Gender and promotions: Evidence from academic economists in France. _Scand. J. Econ._ 121, 1020–1053 (2019). Article  Google Scholar  * Conner, A. L. _et al._ Obscuring


gender bias with “choice”. _Science_ 343, 1200–1200 (2014). Article  ADS  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Fortunato, S. _et al._ Science of science. _Science_ 359, eaao0185 (2018). Article 


PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar  * Kern-Goldberger, A. R., James, R., Berghella, V. & Miller, E. S. The impact of double-blind peer review on gender bias in scientific publishing:


A systematic review. _Am. J. Obstet. Gynecol._ 227, 43-50.e4 (2022). Article  PubMed  Google Scholar  * Angrist, J. D. & Pischke, J.-S. _Mostly Harmless Econometrics: An Empiricist’s


Companion_ (Princeton University Press, 2009). Book  MATH  Google Scholar  * Allison, P. D., Williams, R. A., Hippel, V. Better predicted probabilities from linear probability models.


_Statistical Horizons_. https://statisticalhorizons.com/better-predicted-probabilities/ (2020). * Ai, C. & Norton, E. C. Interaction terms in logit and probit models. _Econ. Lett._ 80,


123–129 (2003). Article  Google Scholar  * Karaca-Mandic, P., Norton, E. C. & Dowd, B. Interaction terms in nonlinear models. _Health Serv. Res._ 47, 255–274 (2012). Article  PubMed 


Google Scholar  Download references ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I would like to acknowledge Ellen de Bruin for inspiring this paper, Hélène Boeren for the collection and harmonizing of the data from


the university registers, Amy Zeegelaar and Niek Brunsveld for discussions on potential explanations of gender inequality in cum laude distinctions, and Sara Geven, Mathijs de Vaan, Arnout


van de Rijt, and participants in seminars at Trinity College, Free University Amsterdam, and the Economics and Sociology Workshop at the University of Amsterdam for providing feedback to


earlier drafts. AUTHOR INFORMATION AUTHORS AND AFFILIATIONS * Department of Sociology, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands Thijs Bol * Amsterdam Centre for Inequality


Studies, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands Thijs Bol Authors * Thijs Bol View author publications You can also search for this author inPubMed Google Scholar CONTRIBUTIONS


T.B. developed the research questions and design, did the analyses, and wrote the article. CORRESPONDING AUTHOR Correspondence to Thijs Bol. ETHICS DECLARATIONS COMPETING INTERESTS The


author declares no competing interests. ADDITIONAL INFORMATION PUBLISHER'S NOTE Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional


affiliations. SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION. RIGHTS AND PERMISSIONS OPEN ACCESS This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License,


which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a


link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence,


unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory


regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit


http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. Reprints and permissions ABOUT THIS ARTICLE CITE THIS ARTICLE Bol, T. Gender inequality in cum laude distinctions for PhD students. _Sci Rep_ 13,


20267 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-023-46375-7 Download citation * Received: 01 June 2023 * Accepted: 31 October 2023 * Published: 29 November 2023 * DOI:


https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-023-46375-7 SHARE THIS ARTICLE Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content: Get shareable link Sorry, a shareable link is not


currently available for this article. Copy to clipboard Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative