Imagination makes us human – this unique ability to envision what doesn’t exist has a long evolutionary history

Imagination makes us human – this unique ability to envision what doesn’t exist has a long evolutionary history

Play all audios:

Loading...

You can easily picture yourself riding a bicycle across the sky even though that’s not something that can actually happen. You can envision yourself doing something you’ve never done before


– like water skiing – and maybe even imagine a better way to do it than anyone else. Imagination involves creating a mental image of something that is not present for your senses to detect,


or even something that isn’t out there in reality somewhere. Imagination is one of the key abilities that make us human. But where did it come from? I’m a neuroscientist who studies how


children acquire imagination. I’m especially interested in the neurological mechanisms of imagination. Once we identify what brain structures and connections are necessary to mentally


construct new objects and scenes, scientists like me can look back over the course of evolution to see when these brain areas emerged – and potentially gave birth to the first kinds of


imagination. FROM BACTERIA TO MAMMALS After life emerged on Earth around 3.4 billion years ago, organisms gradually became more complex. Around 700 million years ago, neurons organized into


simple neural nets that then evolved into the brain and spinal cord around 525 million years ago. Eventually dinosaurs evolved around 240 million years ago, with mammals emerging a few


million years later. While they shared the landscape, dinosaurs were very good at catching and eating small, furry mammals. Dinosaurs were cold-blooded, though, and, like modern cold-blooded


reptiles, could only move and hunt effectively during the daytime when it was warm. To avoid predation by dinosaurs, mammals stumbled upon a solution: hide underground during the daytime.


Not much food, though, grows underground. To eat, mammals had to travel above the ground – but the safest time to forage was at night, when dinosaurs were less of a threat. Evolving to be


warm-blooded meant mammals could move at night. That solution came with a trade-off, though: Mammals had to eat a lot more food than dinosaurs per unit of weight in order to maintain their


high metabolism and to support their constant inner body temperature around 99 degrees Fahrenheit (37 degrees Celsius). Our mammalian ancestors had to find 10 times more food during their


short waking time, and they had to find it in the dark of night. How did they accomplish this task? To optimize their foraging, mammals developed a new system to efficiently memorize places


where they’d found food: linking the part of the brain that records sensory aspects of the landscape – how a place looks or smells – to the part of the brain that controls navigation. They


encoded features of the landscape in the neocortex, the outermost layer of the brain. They encoded navigation in the entorhinal cortex. And the whole system was interconnected by the brain


structure called the hippocampus. Humans still use this memory system for remembering objects and past events, such as your car and where you parked it. Groups of neurons in the neocortex


encode these memories of objects and past events. Remembering a thing or an episode reactivates the same neurons that initially encoded it. All mammals likely can recall and re-experience


previously encoded objects and events by reactivating these groups of neurons. This neocortex-hippocampus-based memory system that evolved 200 million years ago became the first key step


toward imagination. The next building block is the capability to construct a “memory” that hasn’t really happened. INVOLUNTARY MADE-UP ‘MEMORIES’ The simplest form of imagining new objects


and scenes happens in dreams. These vivid, bizarre involuntary fantasies are associated in people with the rapid eye movement (REM) stage of sleep. Scientists hypothesize that species whose


rest includes periods of REM sleep also experience dreams. Marsupial and placental mammals do have REM sleep, but the egg-laying mammal the echidna does not, suggesting that this stage of


the sleep cycle evolved after these evolutionary lines diverged 140 million years ago. In fact, recording from specialized neurons in the brain called place cells demonstrated that animals


can “dream” of going places they’ve never visited before. In humans, solutions found during dreaming can help solve problems. There are numerous examples of scientific and engineering


solutions spontaneously visualized during sleep. The neuroscientist Otto Loewi dreamed of an experiment that proved nerve impulses are transmitted chemically. He immediately went to his lab


to perform the experiment – later receiving the Nobel Prize for this discovery. Elias Howe, the inventor of the first sewing machine, claimed that the main innovation, placing the thread


hole near the tip of the needle, came to him in a dream. Dmitri Mendeleev described seeing in a dream “a table where all the elements fell into place as required. Awakening, I immediately


wrote it down on a piece of paper.” And that was the periodic table. These discoveries were enabled by the same mechanism of involuntary imagination first acquired by mammals 140 million


years ago. IMAGINING ON PURPOSE The difference between voluntary imagination and involuntary imagination is analogous to the difference between voluntary muscle control and muscle spasm.


Voluntary muscle control allows people to deliberately combine muscle movements. Spasm occurs spontaneously and cannot be controlled. Similarly, voluntary imagination allows people to


deliberately combine thoughts. When asked to mentally combine two identical right triangles along their long edges, or hypotenuses, you envision a square. When asked to mentally cut a round


pizza by two perpendicular lines, you visualize four identical slices. This deliberate, responsive and reliable capacity to combine and recombine mental objects is called prefrontal


synthesis. It relies on the ability of the prefrontal cortex located at the very front of the brain to control the rest of the neocortex. When did our species acquire the ability of


prefrontal synthesis? Every artifact dated before 70,000 years ago could have been made by a creator who lacked this ability. On the other hand, starting about that time there are various


archeological artifacts unambiguously indicating its presence: composite figurative objects, such as lion-man; bone needles with an eye; bows and arrows; musical instruments; constructed


dwellings; adorned burials suggesting the beliefs in afterlife, and many more. Multiple types of archaeological artifacts unambiguously associated with prefrontal synthesis appear


simultaneously around 65,000 years ago in multiple geographical locations. This abrupt change in imagination has been characterized by historian Yuval Harari as the “cognitive revolution.”


Notably, it approximately coincides with the largest _Homo sapiens_‘ migration out of Africa. Genetic analyses suggest that a few individuals acquired this prefrontal synthesis ability and


then spread their genes far and wide by eliminating other contemporaneous males with the use of an imagination-enabeled strategy and newly developed weapons. So it’s been a journey of many


millions of years of evolution for our species to become equipped with imagination. Most nonhuman mammals have potential for imagining what doesn’t exist or hasn’t happened involuntarily


during REM sleep; only humans can voluntarily conjure new objects and events in our minds using prefrontal synthesis.